{"publication_id":52,"study_id":84,"es_id":115,"reference":"Diliberti et al. (2004)","title":"Increased portion size leads to increased energy intake in a restaurant meal","year":2004,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":180,"n_comparison":180,"n_control":89,"n_intervention":91,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1671,"sd_control":122.64,"mean_intervention":2390,"sd_intervention":305.26,"cohens_d":3.0784,"variance_d":0.0485,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":66,"study_id":114,"es_id":146,"reference":"Geier et al. (2012)","title":"Red Potato Chips: Segmentation cues can substantially decrease food intake","year":2012,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":39,"n_comparison":27,"n_control":13,"n_intervention":14,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":35,"sd_control":7.77,"mean_intervention":14.45,"sd_intervention":5.9,"cohens_d":2.995,"variance_d":0.2342,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":66,"study_id":114,"es_id":147,"reference":"Geier et al. (2012)","title":"Red Potato Chips: Segmentation cues can substantially decrease food intake","year":2012,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":39,"n_comparison":25,"n_control":13,"n_intervention":12,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":35,"sd_control":7.77,"mean_intervention":15.86,"sd_intervention":5.8,"cohens_d":2.7746,"variance_d":0.3142,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":176,"study_id":272,"es_id":366,"reference":"Steffel et al. (2016)","title":"Ethically deployed defaults: Transparency and consumer protection through disclosure and preference articulation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":105,"n_comparison":105,"n_control":53,"n_intervention":52,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.06,"mean_intervention":1,"cohens_d":2.6467,"variance_d":0.0715,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":194,"study_id":302,"es_id":405,"reference":"Van Kleef et al. (2018)","title":"The effect of a default-based nudge on the choice of whole wheat bread","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":117,"n_comparison":117,"n_control":59,"n_intervention":58,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.19,"mean_intervention":1,"cohens_d":2.2395,"variance_d":0.0556,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":176,"study_id":273,"es_id":367,"reference":"Steffel et al. (2016)","title":"Ethically deployed defaults: Transparency and consumer protection through disclosure and preference articulation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":105,"n_comparison":105,"n_control":53,"n_intervention":52,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.02,"mean_intervention":0.9,"cohens_d":2.2143,"variance_d":0.0614,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":66,"study_id":115,"es_id":148,"reference":"Geier et al. (2012)","title":"Red Potato Chips: Segmentation cues can substantially decrease food intake","year":2012,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":59,"n_comparison":40,"n_control":19,"n_intervention":21,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":45.25,"sd_control":14.02,"mean_intervention":20.25,"sd_intervention":9.26,"cohens_d":2.1263,"variance_d":0.1568,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":200,"study_id":310,"es_id":414,"reference":"Wansink & van Ittersum (2003)","title":"Bottoms up! The influence of elongation on pouring and consumption volume","year":2003,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":97,"n_comparison":97,"n_control":49,"n_intervention":48,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5.54,"mean_intervention":9.66,"cohens_d":1.966,"variance_d":0.0612,"approximation":true,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":111,"study_id":183,"es_id":237,"reference":"Loeb et al. (2017)","title":"The application of defaults to optimize parents' health-based choices for children","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":62,"n_comparison":62,"n_control":28,"n_intervention":34,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.17857,"mean_intervention":0.97059,"cohens_d":1.9243,"variance_d":0.095,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":208,"study_id":325,"es_id":433,"reference":"Young et al. (2009)","title":"Opt-out testing for stigmatized diseases: A social psychological approach to understanding the potential effect of recommendations for routine HIV testing","year":2009,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":40,"n_comparison":40,"n_control":20,"n_intervention":20,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.125,"mean_intervention":0.94,"cohens_d":1.9239,"variance_d":0.1463,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":66,"study_id":115,"es_id":149,"reference":"Geier et al. (2012)","title":"Red Potato Chips: Segmentation cues can substantially decrease food intake","year":2012,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":59,"n_comparison":38,"n_control":19,"n_intervention":19,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":45.25,"sd_control":14.02,"mean_intervention":23.68,"sd_intervention":7.79,"cohens_d":1.9019,"variance_d":0.1529,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":176,"study_id":274,"es_id":368,"reference":"Steffel et al. (2016)","title":"Ethically deployed defaults: Transparency and consumer protection through disclosure and preference articulation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":817,"n_comparison":817,"n_control":409,"n_intervention":408,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5.53,"sd_control":3.42,"mean_intervention":11.22,"sd_intervention":3.38,"cohens_d":1.6735,"variance_d":0.0066,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":200,"study_id":311,"es_id":415,"reference":"Wansink & van Ittersum (2003)","title":"Bottoms up! The influence of elongation on pouring and consumption volume","year":2003,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":45,"n_comparison":45,"n_control":23,"n_intervention":22,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.62,"mean_intervention":2.06,"cohens_d":1.6288,"variance_d":0.1184,"approximation":true,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":169,"study_id":263,"es_id":353,"reference":"Shevchenko et al. (2014)","title":"Change and status quo in decisions with defaults: The effect of incidental emotions depends on the type of default","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":67,"n_comparison":67,"n_control":32,"n_intervention":35,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.13,"mean_intervention":0.83,"cohens_d":1.5539,"variance_d":0.0778,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":85,"study_id":140,"es_id":184,"reference":"Hou (2017)","title":"Can interface cues nudge modeling of food consumption? Experiments on a food-ordering website","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":60,"n_comparison":60,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":30,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":8.59,"sd_control":4.02,"mean_intervention":3.47,"sd_intervention":2.41,"cohens_d":1.5448,"variance_d":0.0866,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":25,"study_id":40,"es_id":57,"reference":"Bohnet et al. (2016)","title":"When performance trumps gender bias: Joint vs separate evaluation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":61,"n_comparison":61,"n_control":26,"n_intervention":35,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.65,"mean_intervention":0.03,"cohens_d":1.5273,"variance_d":0.0862,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":203,"study_id":316,"es_id":420,"reference":"Wansink et al. (2017)","title":"Larger partitions lead to larger sales: Divided grocery carts alter purchase norms and increase sales","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":86,"n_comparison":60,"n_control":34,"n_intervention":26,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":11.61,"sd_control":3.64,"mean_intervention":17.54,"sd_intervention":4.19,"cohens_d":1.5257,"variance_d":0.0873,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":168,"study_id":262,"es_id":352,"reference":"Shealy et al. (2018)","title":"Providing descriptive norms during engineering design can encourage more sustainable infrastructure","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":67,"n_comparison":67,"n_control":24,"n_intervention":43,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":93,"sd_control":37.38,"mean_intervention":140.5,"sd_intervention":28.36,"cohens_d":1.4916,"variance_d":0.0815,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":194,"study_id":303,"es_id":406,"reference":"Van Kleef et al. (2018)","title":"The effect of a default-based nudge on the choice of whole wheat bread","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":109,"n_comparison":109,"n_control":52,"n_intervention":57,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.21,"mean_intervention":0.88,"cohens_d":1.482,"variance_d":0.0469,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":122,"study_id":199,"es_id":265,"reference":"Meeker et al. (2014)","title":"Nudging guideline-concordant antibiotic prescribing: A randomized clinical trial","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":14,"n_comparison":14,"n_control":7,"n_intervention":7,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":9.9,"sd_control":13.63372,"mean_intervention":-9.8,"sd_intervention":13.02628,"cohens_d":1.4775,"variance_d":0.3637,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":111,"study_id":184,"es_id":238,"reference":"Loeb et al. (2017)","title":"The application of defaults to optimize parents' health-based choices for children","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":58,"n_comparison":58,"n_control":22,"n_intervention":36,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.27273,"mean_intervention":0.91667,"cohens_d":1.457,"variance_d":0.0915,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":199,"study_id":308,"es_id":412,"reference":"Wansink & Kim (2005)","title":"Bad popcorn in big buckets: Portion size can influence intake as much as taste","year":2005,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":78,"n_comparison":78,"n_control":38,"n_intervention":40,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":58.9,"sd_control":16.7,"mean_intervention":85.6,"sd_intervention":19.8,"cohens_d":1.4545,"variance_d":0.0649,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":202,"study_id":315,"es_id":419,"reference":"Wansink et al. (2014)","title":"Larger bowl size increases the amount of cereal children request, consume, and waste","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":69,"n_comparison":69,"n_control":35,"n_intervention":34,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":46.1,"sd_control":15.1,"mean_intervention":24.7,"sd_intervention":14.9,"cohens_d":1.4265,"variance_d":0.0727,"approximation":true,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":56,"study_id":94,"es_id":125,"reference":"Ebeling & Lotz (2015)","title":"Domestic uptake of green energy promoted by opt-out tariffs","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":41952,"n_comparison":41952,"n_control":20976,"n_intervention":20976,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.072,"mean_intervention":0.691,"cohens_d":1.4194,"variance_d":0.0001,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":143,"study_id":227,"es_id":306,"reference":"Privitera & Zuraikat (2014)","title":"Proximity of foods in a competitive food environment influences consumption of a low calorie and a high calorie food","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":56,"n_comparison":38,"n_control":21,"n_intervention":17,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":9.5,"sd_control":7,"mean_intervention":2,"sd_intervention":1.6,"cohens_d":1.4083,"variance_d":0.1325,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":143,"study_id":227,"es_id":307,"reference":"Privitera & Zuraikat (2014)","title":"Proximity of foods in a competitive food environment influences consumption of a low calorie and a high calorie food","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":56,"n_comparison":39,"n_control":21,"n_intervention":18,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":30,"sd_control":22.6,"mean_intervention":6.2,"sd_intervention":5.2,"cohens_d":1.4012,"variance_d":0.1283,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":204,"study_id":319,"es_id":426,"reference":"Wansink, Cardello et al. (2005)","title":"Fluid consumption and the potential role of canteen shape in minimizing dehydration","year":2005,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":50,"n_comparison":50,"n_control":25,"n_intervention":25,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":16.64,"sd_control":5.12,"mean_intervention":23.02,"sd_intervention":3.91,"cohens_d":1.4005,"variance_d":0.0996,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":56,"study_id":95,"es_id":126,"reference":"Ebeling & Lotz (2015)","title":"Domestic uptake of green energy promoted by opt-out tariffs","year":2015,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":290,"n_comparison":290,"n_control":145,"n_intervention":145,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.3416,"mean_intervention":0.938,"cohens_d":1.3899,"variance_d":0.0171,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":59,"study_id":101,"es_id":133,"reference":"Everett et al. (2015)","title":"Doing good by doing nothing? The role of social norms in explaining default effects in altruistic context","year":2015,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":143,"n_comparison":143,"n_control":72,"n_intervention":71,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.19,"mean_intervention":0.81,"cohens_d":1.3375,"variance_d":0.0342,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":183,"study_id":290,"es_id":386,"reference":"Taylor et al. (2015)","title":"Improving social norms interventions: Rank-framing increases excessive alcohol drinkers' information-seeking","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":58,"n_comparison":41,"n_control":21,"n_intervention":20,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.048,"mean_intervention":0.6,"cohens_d":1.3304,"variance_d":0.1192,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":98,"study_id":166,"es_id":217,"reference":"Klotz et al. (2010)","title":"Unintended anchors: Building rating systems and energy performance goals for U.S. buildings","year":2010,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":76,"n_comparison":52,"n_control":20,"n_intervention":32,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":29,"sd_control":17.6,"mean_intervention":54,"sd_intervention":20,"cohens_d":1.3073,"variance_d":0.0977,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":103,"study_id":173,"es_id":226,"reference":"Kuester et al. (2015)","title":"The role of defaults in preventing innovation rejection","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":240,"n_comparison":240,"n_control":120,"n_intervention":120,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.317,"mean_intervention":0.9,"cohens_d":1.302,"variance_d":0.0202,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":99,"study_id":167,"es_id":219,"reference":"Knowles et al. (2019)","title":"Exploring the underpinning mechanisms of the proximity effect within a competitive food environment","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":25,"n_comparison":25,"n_control":13,"n_intervention":12,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":11.26,"sd_control":17.72,"mean_intervention":62.83,"sd_intervention":54.24,"cohens_d":1.3012,"variance_d":0.1941,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":8,"study_id":8,"es_id":11,"reference":"Bachman & Katzev (1982)","title":"The effects of non-contingent free bus tickets and personal commitment on urban bus ridership","year":1982,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":37,"n_comparison":37,"n_control":20,"n_intervention":17,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.05,"mean_intervention":0.59,"cohens_d":1.2972,"variance_d":0.1316,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":57,"study_id":96,"es_id":127,"reference":"Engell et al. (1996)","title":"Effects of effort and social modeling on drinking in humans","year":1996,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":36,"n_comparison":24,"n_control":12,"n_intervention":12,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":444,"sd_control":259,"mean_intervention":186.7,"sd_intervention":115.2,"cohens_d":1.2837,"variance_d":0.201,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":57,"study_id":96,"es_id":128,"reference":"Engell et al. (1996)","title":"Effects of effort and social modeling on drinking in humans","year":1996,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":36,"n_comparison":24,"n_control":12,"n_intervention":12,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":444,"sd_control":259,"mean_intervention":197.1,"sd_intervention":100.2,"cohens_d":1.2573,"variance_d":0.1996,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":176,"study_id":275,"es_id":369,"reference":"Steffel et al. (2016)","title":"Ethically deployed defaults: Transparency and consumer protection through disclosure and preference articulation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":779,"n_comparison":779,"n_control":390,"n_intervention":389,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":4.49,"sd_control":3.14,"mean_intervention":8.76,"sd_intervention":3.79,"cohens_d":1.2271,"variance_d":0.0061,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":92,"study_id":153,"es_id":202,"reference":"Johnson et al. (2002)","title":"Defaults, framing and privacy: Why opting in-opting out","year":2002,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":138,"n_comparison":138,"n_control":69,"n_intervention":69,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.482,"mean_intervention":0.963,"cohens_d":1.2197,"variance_d":0.0344,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":204,"study_id":319,"es_id":427,"reference":"Wansink, Cardello et al. (2005)","title":"Fluid consumption and the potential role of canteen shape in minimizing dehydration","year":2005,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":50,"n_comparison":50,"n_control":25,"n_intervention":25,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":16.33,"sd_control":4.3,"mean_intervention":21.81,"sd_intervention":4.85,"cohens_d":1.1957,"variance_d":0.0943,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":16,"study_id":28,"es_id":32,"reference":"Bergeron et al. (2019)","title":"Using insights from behavioral economics to nudge individuals towards healthier choices when eating out: A restaurant experiment","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":98,"n_comparison":98,"n_control":48,"n_intervention":50,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.31,"mean_intervention":0.86,"cohens_d":1.1936,"variance_d":0.0481,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":182,"study_id":289,"es_id":385,"reference":"Tavernier & Adam (2017)","title":"Text message intervention improves objective sleep hours among adolescents: The moderating role of race-ethnicity","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":20,"n_comparison":20,"n_control":10,"n_intervention":10,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":6.68,"sd_control":1.2,"mean_intervention":7.8,"sd_intervention":0.59,"cohens_d":1.1845,"variance_d":0.2351,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":195,"study_id":304,"es_id":407,"reference":"Van Kleef, Shimizu et al. (2012)","title":"Serving bowl selection biases the amount of food served","year":2012,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":67,"n_comparison":67,"n_control":37,"n_intervention":30,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":301.3,"sd_control":129.6,"mean_intervention":176.7,"sd_intervention":72.8,"cohens_d":1.1536,"variance_d":0.0703,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":25,"study_id":41,"es_id":58,"reference":"Bohnet et al. (2016)","title":"When performance trumps gender bias: Joint vs separate evaluation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":61,"n_comparison":61,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":31,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.53,"mean_intervention":0.06,"cohens_d":1.1359,"variance_d":0.0762,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":95,"study_id":158,"es_id":207,"reference":"Keller et al. (2011)","title":"Enhanced active choice: A new method to motivate behavior change","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":110,"n_comparison":55,"n_control":28,"n_intervention":27,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.45,"mean_intervention":0.93,"cohens_d":1.1354,"variance_d":0.0845,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":203,"study_id":317,"es_id":421,"reference":"Wansink et al. (2017)","title":"Larger partitions lead to larger sales: Divided grocery carts alter purchase norms and increase sales","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":107,"n_comparison":73,"n_control":36,"n_intervention":37,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5.61,"sd_control":3.99,"mean_intervention":12.51,"sd_intervention":7.64,"cohens_d":1.1276,"variance_d":0.0635,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":147,"study_id":233,"es_id":314,"reference":"Raynor & Wing (2007)","title":"Package unit size and amount of food: Do both influence intake?","year":2007,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":28,"n_comparison":28,"n_control":14,"n_intervention":14,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2782.2,"sd_control":1174.5,"mean_intervention":5028.1,"sd_intervention":2596.1,"cohens_d":1.1147,"variance_d":0.165,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":88,"study_id":143,"es_id":192,"reference":"Isaksen et al. (2019)","title":"Positive framing does not solve the tragedy of the commons","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":108,"n_comparison":108,"n_control":52,"n_intervention":56,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.26,"sd_control":0.1,"mean_intervention":0.46,"sd_intervention":0.23,"cohens_d":1.1135,"variance_d":0.0428,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":116,"study_id":192,"es_id":255,"reference":"Marchiori et al. (2012)","title":"Container size influences snack food intake independently of portion size","year":2012,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":59,"n_comparison":59,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":29,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":30.4,"sd_control":29.0293,"mean_intervention":69.5,"sd_intervention":43.08132,"cohens_d":1.0609,"variance_d":0.0774,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":4,"study_id":4,"es_id":6,"reference":"Ansher et al. (2014)","title":"Better medicine by default","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":45,"n_comparison":45,"n_control":21,"n_intervention":24,"binary_outcome":false,"cohens_d":1.0553,"variance_d":0.1017,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":101,"study_id":170,"es_id":223,"reference":"Kressel & Chapman (2007)","title":"The default effect in the end-of-life medical treatment preferences","year":2007,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":51,"n_comparison":51,"n_control":25,"n_intervention":26,"binary_outcome":false,"cohens_d":1.033,"variance_d":0.0889,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":141,"study_id":223,"es_id":302,"reference":"Pichert & Katsikopoulos (2008)","title":"Green defaults: Information presentation and pro-environmental behaviour","year":2008,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":65,"n_comparison":65,"n_control":41,"n_intervention":24,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":6.59,"sd_control":5.53,"mean_intervention":13,"sd_intervention":7.34,"cohens_d":1.0253,"variance_d":0.0741,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":8,"study_id":9,"es_id":12,"reference":"Bachman & Katzev (1982)","title":"The effects of non-contingent free bus tickets and personal commitment on urban bus ridership","year":1982,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":38,"n_comparison":38,"n_control":21,"n_intervention":17,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.43,"mean_intervention":0.88,"cohens_d":1.0139,"variance_d":0.12,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":176,"study_id":276,"es_id":370,"reference":"Steffel et al. (2016)","title":"Ethically deployed defaults: Transparency and consumer protection through disclosure and preference articulation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":681,"n_comparison":681,"n_control":341,"n_intervention":340,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":4.57,"sd_control":3.03,"mean_intervention":8.13,"sd_intervention":3.95,"cohens_d":1.0115,"variance_d":0.0066,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":68,"study_id":117,"es_id":151,"reference":"Goldstein et al. (2011)","title":"Reciprocity by proxy: A novel influence strategy for stimulating cooperation","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":79,"n_comparison":79,"n_control":40,"n_intervention":39,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.211,"mean_intervention":0.683,"cohens_d":0.991,"variance_d":0.0569,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":4,"study_id":4,"es_id":7,"reference":"Ansher et al. (2014)","title":"Better medicine by default","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":45,"n_comparison":45,"n_control":24,"n_intervention":21,"binary_outcome":false,"cohens_d":0.9833,"variance_d":0.1,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":198,"study_id":307,"es_id":410,"reference":"Wansink & Hanks (2013)","title":"Slim by design: Serving healthy foods first in buffet lines improves overall meal selection","year":2013,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":124,"n_comparison":124,"n_control":65,"n_intervention":59,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.754,"mean_intervention":0.288,"cohens_d":0.9707,"variance_d":0.0361,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":16,"study_id":29,"es_id":33,"reference":"Bergeron et al. (2019)","title":"Using insights from behavioral economics to nudge individuals towards healthier choices when eating out: A restaurant experiment","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":99,"n_comparison":99,"n_control":51,"n_intervention":48,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.33,"mean_intervention":0.79,"cohens_d":0.9656,"variance_d":0.0452,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":192,"study_id":300,"es_id":402,"reference":"Van der Zanden et al. (2015)","title":"Using a verbal prompt to increase protein consumption in a hospital setting: A field study","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":155,"n_comparison":155,"n_control":93,"n_intervention":62,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.065,"mean_intervention":0.452,"cohens_d":0.9591,"variance_d":0.0298,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":132,"study_id":211,"es_id":284,"reference":"Neale & Bazerman (1985)","title":"The effects of framing and negotiators overconfidence on bargaining behaviors and outcomes","year":1985,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":100,"n_comparison":100,"n_control":50,"n_intervention":50,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.1,"sd_control":1.7,"mean_intervention":-1,"sd_intervention":2.6,"cohens_d":0.956,"variance_d":0.0446,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":73,"study_id":125,"es_id":161,"reference":"Goswami & Urminsky (2016)","title":"When should the ask be a nudge? The effect of default amounts on charitable donations","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":76,"n_comparison":38,"n_control":24,"n_intervention":14,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.54,"mean_intervention":0.93,"cohens_d":0.9552,"variance_d":0.1251,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":162,"study_id":251,"es_id":339,"reference":"Schwartz (2007)","title":"The influence of a verbal prompt on school lunch fruit consumption: A pilot study","year":2007,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":325,"n_comparison":325,"n_control":176,"n_intervention":149,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.65,"mean_intervention":0.97,"cohens_d":0.9179,"variance_d":0.0137,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":187,"study_id":294,"es_id":393,"reference":"Trevana et al. (2006)","title":"Impact of privacy legislation on the number and characteristics of people who are recruited for a research: A randomized controlled trial","year":2006,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":152,"n_comparison":152,"n_control":92,"n_intervention":60,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.5109,"mean_intervention":0.9,"cohens_d":0.9056,"variance_d":0.0302,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":166,"study_id":260,"es_id":350,"reference":"Shealy & Klotz (2015)","title":"Well-endowed rating systems: How modified defaults can lead to more sustainable performance","year":2015,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":41,"n_comparison":41,"n_control":25,"n_intervention":16,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":44.68,"sd_control":19.8,"mean_intervention":62.39,"sd_intervention":19.36,"cohens_d":0.9021,"variance_d":0.1124,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":176,"study_id":277,"es_id":371,"reference":"Steffel et al. (2016)","title":"Ethically deployed defaults: Transparency and consumer protection through disclosure and preference articulation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":390,"n_comparison":390,"n_control":195,"n_intervention":195,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":4.08,"sd_control":2.46,"mean_intervention":6.8,"sd_intervention":3.5,"cohens_d":0.8992,"variance_d":0.0113,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":180,"study_id":283,"es_id":377,"reference":"Tannenbaum et al. (2013)","title":"Partitioning menu items to nudge single-item choice","year":2013,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":198,"n_comparison":198,"n_control":99,"n_intervention":99,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.43,"mean_intervention":0.84,"cohens_d":0.8882,"variance_d":0.0222,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":208,"study_id":326,"es_id":434,"reference":"Young et al. (2009)","title":"Opt-out testing for stigmatized diseases: A social psychological approach to understanding the potential effect of recommendations for routine HIV testing","year":2009,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":38,"n_comparison":38,"n_control":19,"n_intervention":19,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.455,"mean_intervention":0.857,"cohens_d":0.8853,"variance_d":0.1156,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":205,"study_id":320,"es_id":428,"reference":"Wansink, Painter et al. (2005)","title":"Bottomless bowls: Why visual cues of portion size may influence intake","year":2005,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":54,"n_comparison":54,"n_control":23,"n_intervention":31,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":14.7,"sd_control":8.4,"mean_intervention":8.5,"sd_intervention":6.1,"cohens_d":0.8655,"variance_d":0.0827,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":81,"study_id":136,"es_id":179,"reference":"Haward et al. (2012)","title":"Default options and neonatal resuscitation decisions","year":2012,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":291,"n_comparison":291,"n_control":147,"n_intervention":144,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.39,"mean_intervention":0.8,"cohens_d":0.8653,"variance_d":0.015,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":176,"study_id":278,"es_id":372,"reference":"Steffel et al. (2016)","title":"Ethically deployed defaults: Transparency and consumer protection through disclosure and preference articulation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":429,"n_comparison":429,"n_control":215,"n_intervention":214,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":7.03,"sd_control":3.78,"mean_intervention":9.56,"sd_intervention":1.71,"cohens_d":0.8617,"variance_d":0.0102,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":90,"study_id":150,"es_id":199,"reference":"Johnson & Goldstein (2003)","title":"Do defaults save lives?","year":2003,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":108,"n_comparison":108,"n_control":54,"n_intervention":54,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.42,"mean_intervention":0.82,"cohens_d":0.8552,"variance_d":0.0404,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":157,"study_id":245,"es_id":332,"reference":"Samek et al. (2016)","title":"An experimental study of the decision process with interactive technology","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":80,"n_comparison":80,"n_control":40,"n_intervention":40,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":4.63,"sd_control":0.25298,"mean_intervention":4.82,"sd_intervention":0.18974,"cohens_d":0.8497,"variance_d":0.0545,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":199,"study_id":309,"es_id":413,"reference":"Wansink & Kim (2005)","title":"Bad popcorn in big buckets: Portion size can influence intake as much as taste","year":2005,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":79,"n_comparison":79,"n_control":39,"n_intervention":40,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":38,"sd_control":16.1,"mean_intervention":50.8,"sd_intervention":14.1,"cohens_d":0.8466,"variance_d":0.0552,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":203,"study_id":316,"es_id":422,"reference":"Wansink et al. (2017)","title":"Larger partitions lead to larger sales: Divided grocery carts alter purchase norms and increase sales","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":86,"n_comparison":60,"n_control":34,"n_intervention":26,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":11.61,"sd_control":3.64,"mean_intervention":14.97,"sd_intervention":4.42,"cohens_d":0.8411,"variance_d":0.0738,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":89,"study_id":146,"es_id":195,"reference":"Jin (2011)","title":"Improving response rates in web surveys with default setting: The effects of default on web survey participation and permission","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":197,"n_comparison":197,"n_control":95,"n_intervention":102,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.421,"mean_intervention":0.814,"cohens_d":0.8376,"variance_d":0.0221,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":53,"study_id":85,"es_id":116,"reference":"Dinner et al. (2011)","title":"Partitioning default effects: Why people choose not to choose","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":126,"n_comparison":126,"n_control":63,"n_intervention":63,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.19,"mean_intervention":0.58,"cohens_d":0.8294,"variance_d":0.0345,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":180,"study_id":284,"es_id":378,"reference":"Tannenbaum et al. (2013)","title":"Partitioning menu items to nudge single-item choice","year":2013,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":302,"n_comparison":302,"n_control":151,"n_intervention":151,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.3995,"mean_intervention":0.793,"cohens_d":0.8285,"variance_d":0.0144,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":68,"study_id":118,"es_id":152,"reference":"Goldstein et al. (2011)","title":"Reciprocity by proxy: A novel influence strategy for stimulating cooperation","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":45,"n_comparison":45,"n_control":15,"n_intervention":30,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.474,"mean_intervention":0.846,"cohens_d":0.8163,"variance_d":0.1074,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":106,"study_id":178,"es_id":231,"reference":"Larrick & Soll (2008)","title":"The MPG illusion","year":2008,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":171,"n_comparison":171,"n_control":78,"n_intervention":93,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.25,"mean_intervention":0.64,"cohens_d":0.8074,"variance_d":0.0255,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":68,"study_id":119,"es_id":153,"reference":"Goldstein et al. (2011)","title":"Reciprocity by proxy: A novel influence strategy for stimulating cooperation","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":22,"n_comparison":22,"n_control":11,"n_intervention":11,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.444,"mean_intervention":0.818,"cohens_d":0.8015,"variance_d":0.1964,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":114,"study_id":188,"es_id":243,"reference":"Maas et al. (2011)","title":"Do distant foods decrease intake? The effect of food accessibility on consumption","year":2011,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":77,"n_comparison":49,"n_control":23,"n_intervention":26,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":10.3,"sd_control":10.16,"mean_intervention":3.77,"sd_intervention":6.02,"cohens_d":0.7942,"variance_d":0.0884,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":180,"study_id":285,"es_id":379,"reference":"Tannenbaum et al. (2013)","title":"Partitioning menu items to nudge single-item choice","year":2013,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":299,"n_comparison":299,"n_control":150,"n_intervention":149,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.417,"mean_intervention":0.78225,"cohens_d":0.7666,"variance_d":0.0144,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":120,"study_id":196,"es_id":261,"reference":"McCalley & Midden (2002)","title":"Energy conservation through product-integrated feedback: The roles of goal-setting and social orientation","year":2002,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":75,"n_comparison":50,"n_control":25,"n_intervention":25,"binary_outcome":false,"cohens_d":0.7586,"variance_d":0.0858,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":167,"study_id":261,"es_id":351,"reference":"Shealy et al. (2016)","title":"Using framing effects to inform more sustainable infrastructure design decisions","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":65,"n_comparison":65,"n_control":33,"n_intervention":32,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":81,"sd_control":40.1,"mean_intervention":112,"sd_intervention":42.6,"cohens_d":0.7497,"variance_d":0.0659,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":25,"study_id":42,"es_id":59,"reference":"Bohnet et al. (2016)","title":"When performance trumps gender bias: Joint vs separate evaluation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":61,"n_comparison":61,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":31,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.5,"mean_intervention":0.16,"cohens_d":0.7478,"variance_d":0.0702,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":98,"study_id":166,"es_id":218,"reference":"Klotz et al. (2010)","title":"Unintended anchors: Building rating systems and energy performance goals for U.S. buildings","year":2010,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":76,"n_comparison":56,"n_control":24,"n_intervention":32,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":40,"sd_control":16.9,"mean_intervention":54,"sd_intervention":20,"cohens_d":0.747,"variance_d":0.0779,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":154,"study_id":241,"es_id":328,"reference":"Rothman et al. (1999)","title":"The systematic influence of gain- and loss-framed messages on interest in and use of different types of health behavior","year":1999,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":60,"n_comparison":60,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":30,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.37,"mean_intervention":0.73,"cohens_d":0.741,"variance_d":0.0712,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":198,"study_id":307,"es_id":411,"reference":"Wansink & Hanks (2013)","title":"Slim by design: Serving healthy foods first in buffet lines improves overall meal selection","year":2013,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":124,"n_comparison":124,"n_control":65,"n_intervention":59,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.538,"mean_intervention":0.864,"cohens_d":0.7393,"variance_d":0.0345,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":25,"study_id":43,"es_id":60,"reference":"Bohnet et al. (2016)","title":"When performance trumps gender bias: Joint vs separate evaluation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":49,"n_comparison":49,"n_control":20,"n_intervention":29,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.35,"mean_intervention":0.07,"cohens_d":0.7306,"variance_d":0.0899,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":207,"study_id":322,"es_id":430,"reference":"Yeomans & Herberich (2014)","title":"An experimental test of the effect of negative social norms on energy-efficient investments","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":77,"n_comparison":77,"n_control":43,"n_intervention":34,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.326,"mean_intervention":0.676,"cohens_d":0.7151,"variance_d":0.056,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":40,"study_id":70,"es_id":91,"reference":"Catlin & Wang (2013)","title":"Recycling gone bad: When the option to recycle increases resource consumption","year":2013,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":43,"n_comparison":43,"n_control":22,"n_intervention":21,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":9.45,"mean_intervention":27.9,"cohens_d":0.7139,"variance_d":0.099,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":153,"study_id":240,"es_id":327,"reference":"Rosenkranz et al. (2017)","title":"Using behavioral insights to make firms more energy efficient: A field experiment on the effects of improved information","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":339,"n_comparison":339,"n_control":170,"n_intervention":169,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.14,"mean_intervention":0.46,"cohens_d":0.7115,"variance_d":0.0125,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":78,"study_id":133,"es_id":176,"reference":"Halpern et al. (2013)","title":"Default options in advance directives influence how patients set goals for end-of-life care","year":2013,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":89,"n_comparison":89,"n_control":49,"n_intervention":40,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.43,"mean_intervention":0.77,"cohens_d":0.7109,"variance_d":0.0482,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":200,"study_id":312,"es_id":416,"reference":"Wansink & van Ittersum (2003)","title":"Bottoms up! The influence of elongation on pouring and consumption volume","year":2003,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":89,"n_comparison":89,"n_control":45,"n_intervention":44,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5.75,"mean_intervention":6.88,"cohens_d":0.7038,"variance_d":0.0477,"approximation":true,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":92,"study_id":154,"es_id":203,"reference":"Johnson et al. (2002)","title":"Defaults, framing and privacy: Why opting in-opting out","year":2002,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":78,"n_comparison":78,"n_control":39,"n_intervention":39,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.599,"mean_intervention":0.892,"cohens_d":0.7018,"variance_d":0.0544,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":180,"study_id":286,"es_id":380,"reference":"Tannenbaum et al. (2013)","title":"Partitioning menu items to nudge single-item choice","year":2013,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":154,"n_comparison":154,"n_control":77,"n_intervention":77,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.39575,"mean_intervention":0.736,"cohens_d":0.7016,"variance_d":0.0276,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":15,"study_id":19,"es_id":23,"reference":"Basu & Savani (2017)","title":"Choosing one at a time? Presenting options simultaneously helps people make more optimal decisions than presenting options sequentially","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":89,"n_comparison":89,"n_control":45,"n_intervention":44,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":44,"mean_intervention":68.18,"cohens_d":0.7003,"variance_d":0.0477,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":193,"study_id":301,"es_id":403,"reference":"Van Kleef et al. (2012)","title":"Healthy snacks at the checkout counter: A lab and field study on the impact of shelf arrangement and assortment structure on consumer choice","year":2012,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":158,"n_comparison":158,"n_control":79,"n_intervention":79,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.139,"mean_intervention":0.443,"cohens_d":0.6924,"variance_d":0.0268,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":5,"study_id":5,"es_id":8,"reference":"Antonuk & Block (2006)","title":"The effect of single serving versus entire package nutritional information on consumption norms and actual consumption of a snack food","year":2006,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":67,"n_comparison":67,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":37,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":33.03,"sd_control":17.72,"mean_intervention":20.81,"sd_intervention":17.71,"cohens_d":0.6898,"variance_d":0.0639,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":92,"study_id":155,"es_id":204,"reference":"Johnson et al. (2002)","title":"Defaults, framing and privacy: Why opting in-opting out","year":2002,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":78,"n_comparison":78,"n_control":39,"n_intervention":39,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.442,"mean_intervention":0.769,"cohens_d":0.6843,"variance_d":0.0543,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":177,"study_id":280,"es_id":374,"reference":"Stephen & Lehmann (2016)","title":"How word-of-mouth transmission encouragement affects consumers' transmission decisions, receiver selection, and diffusion speed","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":60,"n_comparison":60,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":30,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":3.21,"mean_intervention":3.84,"cohens_d":0.6836,"variance_d":0.0706,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":152,"study_id":239,"es_id":325,"reference":"Rohlfs Dominguez et al. (2013)","title":"Providing choice increases children's vegetable intake","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":150,"n_comparison":94,"n_control":44,"n_intervention":50,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":22.36,"sd_control":24.681,"mean_intervention":47.86,"sd_intervention":46.597,"cohens_d":0.6717,"variance_d":0.0451,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":58,"study_id":98,"es_id":130,"reference":"Evans et al. (2011)","title":"Trust and self-control: The moderating role of the default","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":62,"n_comparison":62,"n_control":31,"n_intervention":31,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":8.1,"sd_control":6.03,"mean_intervention":12.26,"sd_intervention":6.42,"cohens_d":0.6679,"variance_d":0.0681,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":201,"study_id":313,"es_id":417,"reference":"Wansink et al. (2006)","title":"Ice cream illusions: Bowls, spoons, and self-served portion sizes","year":2006,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":37,"n_comparison":37,"n_control":17,"n_intervention":20,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5.81,"sd_control":2.26,"mean_intervention":4.38,"sd_intervention":2.05,"cohens_d":0.6656,"variance_d":0.1148,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":30,"study_id":50,"es_id":68,"reference":"Bucher et al. (2013)","title":"Vegetable variety: An effective strategy to increase vegetable choice in children","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":100,"n_comparison":68,"n_control":34,"n_intervention":34,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5.6,"sd_control":6.3,"mean_intervention":10.9,"sd_intervention":9.5,"cohens_d":0.6575,"variance_d":0.062,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":64,"study_id":110,"es_id":142,"reference":"Ganzach & Karsahi (1995)","title":"Message framing and buying behavior: A field experiment","year":1995,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":117,"n_comparison":117,"n_control":55,"n_intervention":62,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.236,"mean_intervention":0.548,"cohens_d":0.6524,"variance_d":0.0361,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":117,"study_id":193,"es_id":256,"reference":"Marek (2018)","title":"Social learning under the labeling effect: Exploring travellers' behavior in social dilemmas","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":120,"n_comparison":120,"n_control":60,"n_intervention":60,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":10.27667,"sd_control":2.23635,"mean_intervention":11.76502,"sd_intervention":2.35069,"cohens_d":0.6487,"variance_d":0.0351,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":201,"study_id":314,"es_id":418,"reference":"Wansink et al. (2006)","title":"Ice cream illusions: Bowls, spoons, and self-served portion sizes","year":2006,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":48,"n_comparison":48,"n_control":22,"n_intervention":26,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":6.58,"sd_control":2.91,"mean_intervention":5.07,"sd_intervention":1.84,"cohens_d":0.6321,"variance_d":0.0881,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":79,"study_id":134,"es_id":177,"reference":"Handgraaf et al. (2013)","title":"Public praise vs. private pay: Effects of rewards on energy conservation in the workplace","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":32,"n_comparison":32,"n_control":16,"n_intervention":16,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.43,"sd_control":4.27,"mean_intervention":3.74,"sd_intervention":6.05,"cohens_d":0.6321,"variance_d":0.1312,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":91,"study_id":151,"es_id":200,"reference":"Johnson et al. (1993)","title":"Framing, probability distortions, and insurance decisions","year":1993,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":136,"n_comparison":136,"n_control":74,"n_intervention":62,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.23,"mean_intervention":0.53,"cohens_d":0.6305,"variance_d":0.0311,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":159,"study_id":247,"es_id":334,"reference":"Saulais et al. (2016)","title":"When are \"Dish of the Day\" nudges most effective to increase vegetable selection?","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":120,"n_comparison":120,"n_control":60,"n_intervention":60,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.233,"mean_intervention":0.533,"cohens_d":0.6294,"variance_d":0.035,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":209,"study_id":327,"es_id":435,"reference":"Zarghamee et al. (2017)","title":"Nudging charitable giving: Three field experiments","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":187,"n_comparison":187,"n_control":69,"n_intervention":118,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.51,"sd_control":0.41,"mean_intervention":0.76,"sd_intervention":0.39,"cohens_d":0.629,"variance_d":0.024,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":28,"study_id":48,"es_id":65,"reference":"Brook & Servatka (2016)","title":"The anticipatory effect of nonverbal communication","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":94,"n_comparison":62,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":32,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2.67,"sd_control":2.15,"mean_intervention":4.09,"sd_intervention":2.37,"cohens_d":0.6266,"variance_d":0.0677,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":63,"study_id":109,"es_id":141,"reference":"Frydman & Rangel (2014)","title":"Debiasing the disposition effect by reducing the saliency of information about a stock's purchase price","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":58,"n_comparison":58,"n_control":33,"n_intervention":25,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":6.8,"mean_intervention":-9,"cohens_d":0.6247,"variance_d":0.0737,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":30,"study_id":50,"es_id":69,"reference":"Bucher et al. (2013)","title":"Vegetable variety: An effective strategy to increase vegetable choice in children","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":100,"n_comparison":66,"n_control":32,"n_intervention":34,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5.9,"sd_control":6.5,"mean_intervention":10.9,"sd_intervention":9.5,"cohens_d":0.6108,"variance_d":0.0635,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":126,"study_id":204,"es_id":274,"reference":"Meyerowitz & Chaiken (1987)","title":"The effect of message framing on breast self-examination, attitudes, intentions, and behavior","year":1987,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":44,"n_comparison":44,"n_control":21,"n_intervention":23,"binary_outcome":false,"cohens_d":0.6091,"variance_d":0.0953,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":96,"study_id":163,"es_id":214,"reference":"Kersbergen et al. (2018)","title":"Reducing the standard serving size of alcoholic beverages prompts reductions in alcohol consumption","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":164,"n_comparison":164,"n_control":77,"n_intervention":87,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2.93,"sd_control":2.19374,"mean_intervention":1.7,"sd_intervention":1.86548,"cohens_d":0.6071,"variance_d":0.0256,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":132,"study_id":211,"es_id":285,"reference":"Neale & Bazerman (1985)","title":"The effects of framing and negotiators overconfidence on bargaining behaviors and outcomes","year":1985,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":100,"n_comparison":100,"n_control":50,"n_intervention":50,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.786,"sd_control":2.3,"mean_intervention":-0.755,"sd_intervention":2.8,"cohens_d":0.6014,"variance_d":0.0418,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":120,"study_id":196,"es_id":262,"reference":"McCalley & Midden (2002)","title":"Energy conservation through product-integrated feedback: The roles of goal-setting and social orientation","year":2002,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":75,"n_comparison":50,"n_control":25,"n_intervention":25,"binary_outcome":false,"cohens_d":0.5971,"variance_d":0.0836,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":171,"study_id":265,"es_id":355,"reference":"Small & Loewenstein (2003)","title":"Helping a victim or helping the victim: Altruism and identifiability","year":2003,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":76,"n_comparison":76,"n_control":37,"n_intervention":39,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2.12,"sd_control":1.79,"mean_intervention":3.42,"sd_intervention":2.51,"cohens_d":0.5937,"variance_d":0.055,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":24,"study_id":37,"es_id":54,"reference":"Bohm et al. (2016)","title":"Outcome valence and externality valence framing in public good dilemmas","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":160,"n_comparison":160,"n_control":80,"n_intervention":80,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":6.63,"sd_control":4.69,"mean_intervention":9.63,"sd_intervention":5.4,"cohens_d":0.5932,"variance_d":0.0261,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":128,"study_id":206,"es_id":277,"reference":"Miller et al. (2016)","title":"The effects of pre-ordering and behavioral nudges on national school lunch program participants' food item selection","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":133,"n_comparison":133,"n_control":98,"n_intervention":35,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.601,"mean_intervention":0.855,"cohens_d":0.5861,"variance_d":0.0401,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":54,"study_id":88,"es_id":119,"reference":"Dogruel et al. (2017)","title":"The valuation of privacy premium features for smartphone apps: The influence of defaults and expert recommendations","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":126,"n_comparison":126,"n_control":62,"n_intervention":64,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.6,"sd_control":1.44,"mean_intervention":2.4,"sd_intervention":1.3,"cohens_d":0.5837,"variance_d":0.0331,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":203,"study_id":317,"es_id":423,"reference":"Wansink et al. (2017)","title":"Larger partitions lead to larger sales: Divided grocery carts alter purchase norms and increase sales","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":107,"n_comparison":70,"n_control":36,"n_intervention":34,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5.61,"sd_control":3.99,"mean_intervention":8.26,"sd_intervention":5.23,"cohens_d":0.5719,"variance_d":0.0595,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":89,"study_id":147,"es_id":196,"reference":"Jin (2011)","title":"Improving response rates in web surveys with default setting: The effects of default on web survey participation and permission","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":124,"n_comparison":124,"n_control":62,"n_intervention":62,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.354,"mean_intervention":0.636,"cohens_d":0.5718,"variance_d":0.0336,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":152,"study_id":239,"es_id":326,"reference":"Rohlfs Dominguez et al. (2013)","title":"Providing choice increases children's vegetable intake","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":150,"n_comparison":100,"n_control":44,"n_intervention":56,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":22.36,"sd_control":24.681,"mean_intervention":43.25,"sd_intervention":44.578,"cohens_d":0.5618,"variance_d":0.0422,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":65,"study_id":112,"es_id":144,"reference":"Gartner (2018)","title":"The prosociality of intuitive decision depends on the status quo","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":405,"n_comparison":405,"n_control":269,"n_intervention":136,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.4981,"mean_intervention":0.7647,"cohens_d":0.5617,"variance_d":0.0115,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":26,"study_id":44,"es_id":61,"reference":"Broman et al. (2014)","title":"The importance of framing for consumer acceptance of the smart Grid: A comparative study of Denmark, Norway, and Switzerland","year":2014,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":945,"n_comparison":945,"n_control":473,"n_intervention":472,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.47,"mean_intervention":0.74,"cohens_d":0.5607,"variance_d":0.0044,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":54,"study_id":89,"es_id":120,"reference":"Dogruel et al. (2017)","title":"The valuation of privacy premium features for smartphone apps: The influence of defaults and expert recommendations","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":116,"n_comparison":116,"n_control":63,"n_intervention":53,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.89,"sd_control":1.37,"mean_intervention":2.64,"sd_intervention":1.3,"cohens_d":0.5603,"variance_d":0.0361,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":57,"study_id":97,"es_id":129,"reference":"Engell et al. (1996)","title":"Effects of effort and social modeling on drinking in humans","year":1996,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":60,"n_comparison":60,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":30,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":307,"sd_control":149,"mean_intervention":226,"sd_intervention":142,"cohens_d":0.5565,"variance_d":0.0692,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":95,"study_id":162,"es_id":213,"reference":"Keller et al. (2015)","title":"Nudging product choices: The effect of position change on snack bar choice","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":120,"n_comparison":120,"n_control":60,"n_intervention":60,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.133,"mean_intervention":0.367,"cohens_d":0.555,"variance_d":0.0346,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":104,"study_id":175,"es_id":228,"reference":"Kulendran et al. (2016)","title":"The use of commitment techniques to support weight loss maintenance in obese adolescents","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":27,"n_comparison":27,"n_control":13,"n_intervention":14,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.06,"sd_control":2.34361,"mean_intervention":-0.12,"sd_intervention":1.94566,"cohens_d":0.5499,"variance_d":0.154,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":142,"study_id":225,"es_id":304,"reference":"Prinsen et al. (2013)","title":"Eating by example. Effects of environmental cues on dietary decisions","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":65,"n_comparison":65,"n_control":33,"n_intervention":32,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.45,"mean_intervention":0.72,"cohens_d":0.5439,"variance_d":0.0638,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":118,"study_id":194,"es_id":257,"reference":"Martin & Norton (2009)","title":"Shaping online consumer choice by partitioning the web","year":2009,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":124,"n_comparison":83,"n_control":42,"n_intervention":41,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.84,"mean_intervention":0.98,"cohens_d":0.5392,"variance_d":0.05,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":164,"study_id":255,"es_id":343,"reference":"Sharif & Shu (2021)","title":"Nudging persistence after failure through emergency reserves","year":2021,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":184,"n_comparison":120,"n_control":60,"n_intervention":60,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":3.11,"sd_control":1.58,"mean_intervention":4,"sd_intervention":1.73,"cohens_d":0.5372,"variance_d":0.0345,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":10,"study_id":11,"es_id":15,"reference":"Baek et al. (2014)","title":"Changing the default setting for information privacy protection: What and whose personal information can be better protected?","year":2014,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":445,"n_comparison":445,"n_control":216,"n_intervention":229,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.46,"mean_intervention":0.72,"cohens_d":0.5357,"variance_d":0.0093,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":68,"study_id":120,"es_id":154,"reference":"Goldstein et al. (2011)","title":"Reciprocity by proxy: A novel influence strategy for stimulating cooperation","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":81,"n_comparison":81,"n_control":41,"n_intervention":40,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.205,"mean_intervention":0.452,"cohens_d":0.5349,"variance_d":0.0512,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":46,"study_id":77,"es_id":98,"reference":"Coventry et al. (2016)","title":"Personality and social framing in privacy decision-making: A study on cookie acceptance","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":290,"n_comparison":196,"n_control":88,"n_intervention":108,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.78,"mean_intervention":0.53,"cohens_d":0.5343,"variance_d":0.0214,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":131,"study_id":210,"es_id":283,"reference":"Narula et al. (2014)","title":"Increasing colonoscopies? A psychological perspective on opting in versus opting out","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":81,"n_comparison":81,"n_control":41,"n_intervention":40,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.854,"mean_intervention":0.625,"cohens_d":0.534,"variance_d":0.0512,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":50,"study_id":81,"es_id":105,"reference":"Demarque et al. (2015)","title":"Nudging sustainable consumption: The use of descriptive norms to promote a minority behavior in a realistic online shopping environment","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":122,"n_comparison":61,"n_control":29,"n_intervention":32,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2,"sd_control":2.45,"mean_intervention":3.25,"sd_intervention":2.24,"cohens_d":0.5337,"variance_d":0.0681,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":141,"study_id":224,"es_id":303,"reference":"Pichert & Katsikopoulos (2008)","title":"Green defaults: Information presentation and pro-environmental behaviour","year":2008,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":152,"n_comparison":152,"n_control":75,"n_intervention":77,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.4133,"mean_intervention":0.6753,"cohens_d":0.5325,"variance_d":0.0273,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":125,"study_id":202,"es_id":268,"reference":"Mertens et al. (2020)","title":"This way, please: Uncovering the directional effects of attribute translations on decision making","year":2020,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":356,"n_comparison":193,"n_control":104,"n_intervention":89,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.50537,"mean_intervention":0.75585,"cohens_d":0.5264,"variance_d":0.0216,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":165,"study_id":259,"es_id":349,"reference":"Sharp & Sobal (2012)","title":"Using plate mapping to examine sensitivity to plate size in food portions and meal composition among college students","year":2012,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":270,"n_comparison":270,"n_control":102,"n_intervention":168,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":25.2,"sd_control":10.5,"mean_intervention":31.8,"sd_intervention":13.8,"cohens_d":0.5214,"variance_d":0.0163,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":170,"study_id":264,"es_id":354,"reference":"Shimizu et al. (2010)","title":"When snacks become meals: How hunger and environmental cues bias food intake","year":2010,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":122,"n_comparison":122,"n_control":61,"n_intervention":61,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":531.79,"sd_control":246.92,"mean_intervention":416.39,"sd_intervention":192.92,"cohens_d":0.5208,"variance_d":0.0339,"approximation":true,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":15,"study_id":20,"es_id":24,"reference":"Basu & Savani (2017)","title":"Choosing one at a time? Presenting options simultaneously helps people make more optimal decisions than presenting options sequentially","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":294,"n_comparison":294,"n_control":147,"n_intervention":147,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":46.29,"sd_control":30.04,"mean_intervention":62.66,"sd_intervention":33.1,"cohens_d":0.5179,"variance_d":0.0141,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":53,"study_id":86,"es_id":117,"reference":"Dinner et al. (2011)","title":"Partitioning default effects: Why people choose not to choose","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":190,"n_comparison":190,"n_control":95,"n_intervention":95,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.202,"mean_intervention":0.438,"cohens_d":0.5142,"variance_d":0.0217,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":135,"study_id":216,"es_id":295,"reference":"Nyer & Dellande (2010)","title":"Public commitment as a motivator for weight loss","year":2010,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":142,"n_comparison":142,"n_control":73,"n_intervention":69,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":90.05,"mean_intervention":97.06,"cohens_d":0.514,"variance_d":0.0291,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":159,"study_id":248,"es_id":335,"reference":"Saulais et al. (2016)","title":"When are \"Dish of the Day\" nudges most effective to increase vegetable selection?","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":173,"n_comparison":118,"n_control":61,"n_intervention":57,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.344,"mean_intervention":0.596,"cohens_d":0.5105,"variance_d":0.035,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":123,"study_id":200,"es_id":266,"reference":"Mehta et al. (2018)","title":"Opt-in versus opt-out colorectal cancer screening outreach: A randomized clinical trial","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":314,"n_comparison":314,"n_control":156,"n_intervention":158,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.096,"mean_intervention":0.291,"cohens_d":0.5095,"variance_d":0.0132,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":73,"study_id":125,"es_id":162,"reference":"Goswami & Urminsky (2016)","title":"When should the ask be a nudge? The effect of default amounts on charitable donations","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":76,"n_comparison":48,"n_control":24,"n_intervention":24,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.125,"mean_intervention":0.33,"cohens_d":0.5011,"variance_d":0.0859,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":184,"study_id":291,"es_id":388,"reference":"Theotokis & Manganari (2015)","title":"The impact of choice architecture on consumer behavior: The role of guilt","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":107,"n_comparison":72,"n_control":36,"n_intervention":36,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.56,"mean_intervention":0.79,"cohens_d":0.4984,"variance_d":0.0573,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":108,"study_id":180,"es_id":233,"reference":"Libotte et al. (2014)","title":"The influence of plate size on meal composition. Literature review and experiment","year":2014,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":83,"n_comparison":83,"n_control":42,"n_intervention":41,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":161,"sd_control":74,"mean_intervention":203,"sd_intervention":97,"cohens_d":0.4876,"variance_d":0.0496,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":53,"study_id":87,"es_id":118,"reference":"Dinner et al. (2011)","title":"Partitioning default effects: Why people choose not to choose","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":126,"n_comparison":126,"n_control":63,"n_intervention":63,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.236,"mean_intervention":0.465,"cohens_d":0.4862,"variance_d":0.0327,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":164,"study_id":256,"es_id":344,"reference":"Sharif & Shu (2021)","title":"Nudging persistence after failure through emergency reserves","year":2021,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":198,"n_comparison":198,"n_control":100,"n_intervention":98,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.64,"mean_intervention":0.847,"cohens_d":0.4832,"variance_d":0.0208,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":91,"study_id":152,"es_id":201,"reference":"Johnson et al. (1993)","title":"Framing, probability distortions, and insurance decisions","year":1993,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":187,"n_comparison":187,"n_control":94,"n_intervention":93,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.443,"mean_intervention":0.678,"cohens_d":0.4782,"variance_d":0.022,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":197,"study_id":306,"es_id":409,"reference":"Verplanken & Weenig (1993)","title":"Graphical energy labels and consumers' decisions about home appliances: A process tracing approach","year":1993,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":60,"n_comparison":60,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":30,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.27,"mean_intervention":0.5,"cohens_d":0.478,"variance_d":0.0343,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":161,"study_id":250,"es_id":338,"reference":"Schulz et al. (2018)","title":"Nudging generosity: Choice architecture and cognitive factors in charitable giving","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":869,"n_comparison":869,"n_control":428,"n_intervention":441,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.219,"mean_intervention":0.439,"cohens_d":0.4745,"variance_d":0.0047,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":88,"study_id":144,"es_id":193,"reference":"Isaksen et al. (2019)","title":"Positive framing does not solve the tragedy of the commons","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":156,"n_comparison":156,"n_control":80,"n_intervention":76,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":-0.13,"sd_control":0.25,"mean_intervention":-0.03,"sd_intervention":0.16,"cohens_d":0.4739,"variance_d":0.0264,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":71,"study_id":123,"es_id":158,"reference":"Gong et al. (2017)","title":"Testing the scenario hypothesis: An experimental comparison of scenarios and forecasts for decision support in a complex decision environment","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":175,"n_comparison":175,"n_control":88,"n_intervention":87,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.08,"mean_intervention":0.25,"cohens_d":0.4737,"variance_d":0.0235,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":176,"study_id":279,"es_id":373,"reference":"Steffel et al. (2016)","title":"Ethically deployed defaults: Transparency and consumer protection through disclosure and preference articulation","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":363,"n_comparison":363,"n_control":182,"n_intervention":181,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":6.41,"sd_control":4.1,"mean_intervention":4.81,"sd_intervention":2.47,"cohens_d":0.4724,"variance_d":0.0113,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":125,"study_id":203,"es_id":269,"reference":"Mertens et al. (2020)","title":"This way, please: Uncovering the directional effects of attribute translations on decision making","year":2020,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":345,"n_comparison":168,"n_control":79,"n_intervention":89,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.49766,"mean_intervention":0.72515,"cohens_d":0.4718,"variance_d":0.0246,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":101,"study_id":171,"es_id":224,"reference":"Kressel & Chapman (2007)","title":"The default effect in the end-of-life medical treatment preferences","year":2007,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":128,"n_comparison":128,"n_control":80,"n_intervention":48,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.5,"sd_control":0.23,"mean_intervention":0.6,"sd_intervention":0.18,"cohens_d":0.4701,"variance_d":0.0342,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":181,"study_id":288,"es_id":382,"reference":"Tasoff & Letzler (2014)","title":"Everyone believes in redemption: Nudges and overoptimism in costly task completion","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":310,"n_comparison":198,"n_control":139,"n_intervention":59,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.31,"mean_intervention":0.54,"cohens_d":0.4699,"variance_d":0.0247,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":50,"study_id":82,"es_id":106,"reference":"Demarque et al. (2015)","title":"Nudging sustainable consumption: The use of descriptive norms to promote a minority behavior in a realistic online shopping environment","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":235,"n_comparison":78,"n_control":41,"n_intervention":37,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.56,"sd_control":2.16,"mean_intervention":2.57,"sd_intervention":2.15,"cohens_d":0.4686,"variance_d":0.0528,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":138,"study_id":219,"es_id":298,"reference":"Paese (1995)","title":"Effects of framing on actual time allocation decisions","year":1995,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":111,"n_comparison":111,"n_control":56,"n_intervention":55,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.16,"mean_intervention":0.36,"cohens_d":0.464,"variance_d":0.037,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":142,"study_id":226,"es_id":305,"reference":"Prinsen et al. (2013)","title":"Eating by example. Effects of environmental cues on dietary decisions","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":90,"n_comparison":90,"n_control":45,"n_intervention":45,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.27,"mean_intervention":0.49,"cohens_d":0.4633,"variance_d":0.0456,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":15,"study_id":21,"es_id":25,"reference":"Basu & Savani (2017)","title":"Choosing one at a time? Presenting options simultaneously helps people make more optimal decisions than presenting options sequentially","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":183,"n_comparison":183,"n_control":92,"n_intervention":91,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":35.82,"sd_control":25.21,"mean_intervention":50.43,"sd_intervention":37.33,"cohens_d":0.4592,"variance_d":0.0224,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":23,"study_id":36,"es_id":53,"reference":"Bogliacino & Forero (2015)","title":"Behaviorally designed treatments that increase willingness to treatment from families with children suffering from autism spectrum disorder","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":76,"n_comparison":76,"n_control":38,"n_intervention":38,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.5789,"mean_intervention":0.7895,"cohens_d":0.459,"variance_d":0.054,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":62,"study_id":108,"es_id":140,"reference":"Fisher (2018)","title":"Nutrition labels reduces valuations of food through multiple health and taste channels","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":87,"n_comparison":87,"n_control":44,"n_intervention":43,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.96,"sd_control":0.54,"mean_intervention":0.71,"sd_intervention":0.55,"cohens_d":0.4587,"variance_d":0.0472,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":190,"study_id":298,"es_id":397,"reference":"Van Bavel et al. (2019)","title":"Using protection motivation theory in the design of nudges to improve online security behavior","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":2024,"n_comparison":1015,"n_control":507,"n_intervention":508,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":34.41,"sd_control":13.33,"mean_intervention":28.03,"sd_intervention":14.54,"cohens_d":0.4574,"variance_d":0.004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":11,"study_id":12,"es_id":16,"reference":"Bamberg (2002)","title":"Effects of implementation intention on the actual performance of new environmentally friendly behaviours - Results of two field experiments","year":2002,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":90,"n_comparison":90,"n_control":45,"n_intervention":45,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.311,"mean_intervention":0.533,"cohens_d":0.4541,"variance_d":0.0456,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":15,"study_id":22,"es_id":26,"reference":"Basu & Savani (2017)","title":"Choosing one at a time? Presenting options simultaneously helps people make more optimal decisions than presenting options sequentially","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":395,"n_comparison":395,"n_control":198,"n_intervention":197,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":25.25,"sd_control":22.9,"mean_intervention":36.78,"sd_intervention":27.92,"cohens_d":0.4517,"variance_d":0.0104,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":179,"study_id":282,"es_id":376,"reference":"Stok et al. (2014)","title":"Don't tell me what I should do, but what others do: The influence of descriptive and injunctive peer norms on fruit consumption in adolescents","year":2014,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":54,"n_comparison":54,"n_control":27,"n_intervention":27,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.7,"sd_control":1,"mean_intervention":2.3,"sd_intervention":1.6,"cohens_d":0.4497,"variance_d":0.0759,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":67,"study_id":116,"es_id":150,"reference":"Gerend (2009)","title":"Does calorie information promote lower calorie fast food choices among college students?","year":2009,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":177,"n_comparison":177,"n_control":89,"n_intervention":88,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":934,"sd_control":371,"mean_intervention":788,"sd_intervention":274,"cohens_d":0.4473,"variance_d":0.0232,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":43,"study_id":73,"es_id":94,"reference":"Cheung et al. (2017)","title":"The Hunger Games: Using hunger to promote healthy choices in self-control conflicts","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":94,"n_comparison":94,"n_control":47,"n_intervention":47,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2.43,"sd_control":1.64536,"mean_intervention":3.16,"sd_intervention":1.64536,"cohens_d":0.4437,"variance_d":0.0436,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":180,"study_id":287,"es_id":381,"reference":"Tannenbaum et al. (2013)","title":"Partitioning menu items to nudge single-item choice","year":2013,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":199,"n_comparison":199,"n_control":100,"n_intervention":99,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.07,"mean_intervention":0.22,"cohens_d":0.4409,"variance_d":0.0206,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":190,"study_id":298,"es_id":398,"reference":"Van Bavel et al. (2019)","title":"Using protection motivation theory in the design of nudges to improve online security behavior","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":2024,"n_comparison":1012,"n_control":507,"n_intervention":505,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":34.41,"sd_control":13.33,"mean_intervention":28.11,"sd_intervention":15.33,"cohens_d":0.4386,"variance_d":0.004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":114,"study_id":188,"es_id":244,"reference":"Maas et al. (2011)","title":"Do distant foods decrease intake? The effect of food accessibility on consumption","year":2011,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":77,"n_comparison":51,"n_control":23,"n_intervention":28,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":10.3,"sd_control":10.16,"mean_intervention":5.36,"sd_intervention":12.12,"cohens_d":0.4379,"variance_d":0.0811,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":96,"study_id":164,"es_id":215,"reference":"Kersbergen et al. (2018)","title":"Reducing the standard serving size of alcoholic beverages prompts reductions in alcohol consumption","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":114,"n_comparison":114,"n_control":60,"n_intervention":54,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":3.87,"sd_control":2.01395,"mean_intervention":3.07,"sd_intervention":1.61666,"cohens_d":0.4356,"variance_d":0.036,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":51,"study_id":83,"es_id":113,"reference":"Dickerson et al. (1992)","title":"Using cognitive dissonanace to encourage water conservation","year":1992,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":60,"n_comparison":40,"n_control":20,"n_intervention":20,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":301.8,"sd_control":142.32,"mean_intervention":247.7,"sd_intervention":104.05,"cohens_d":0.434,"variance_d":0.1024,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":103,"study_id":174,"es_id":227,"reference":"Kuester et al. (2015)","title":"The role of defaults in preventing innovation rejection","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":65,"n_comparison":65,"n_control":34,"n_intervention":31,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.5,"mean_intervention":0.71,"cohens_d":0.4334,"variance_d":0.0631,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":73,"study_id":126,"es_id":163,"reference":"Goswami & Urminsky (2016)","title":"When should the ask be a nudge? The effect of default amounts on charitable donations","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":105,"n_comparison":70,"n_control":35,"n_intervention":35,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.19,"mean_intervention":0.38,"cohens_d":0.4264,"variance_d":0.0584,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":2,"study_id":2,"es_id":3,"reference":"Alemany et al. (2019)","title":"Enhancing the privacy risk awareness of teenagers in online social networks through soft-paternalism mechanisms","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":42,"n_comparison":26,"n_control":14,"n_intervention":12,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.457,"sd_control":0.366,"mean_intervention":0.613,"sd_intervention":0.367,"cohens_d":0.4257,"variance_d":0.1582,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":65,"study_id":113,"es_id":145,"reference":"Gartner (2018)","title":"The prosociality of intuitive decision depends on the status quo","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":413,"n_comparison":413,"n_control":272,"n_intervention":141,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.511,"mean_intervention":0.7163,"cohens_d":0.4254,"variance_d":0.011,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":26,"study_id":45,"es_id":62,"reference":"Broman et al. (2014)","title":"The importance of framing for consumer acceptance of the smart Grid: A comparative study of Denmark, Norway, and Switzerland","year":2014,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":956,"n_comparison":956,"n_control":478,"n_intervention":478,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.6,"mean_intervention":0.79,"cohens_d":0.4174,"variance_d":0.0043,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":3,"study_id":3,"es_id":5,"reference":"Alinia et al. (2011)","title":"A workplace feasibility study of the effect of a minimal fruit intervention on fruit intake","year":2011,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":124,"n_comparison":124,"n_control":56,"n_intervention":68,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":10,"sd_control":179.5996,"mean_intervention":112,"sd_intervention":288.6174,"cohens_d":0.4154,"variance_d":0.0333,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":164,"study_id":255,"es_id":345,"reference":"Sharif & Shu (2021)","title":"Nudging persistence after failure through emergency reserves","year":2021,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":184,"n_comparison":124,"n_control":60,"n_intervention":64,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":3.11,"sd_control":1.58,"mean_intervention":3.82,"sd_intervention":1.85,"cohens_d":0.4117,"variance_d":0.033,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":133,"study_id":212,"es_id":286,"reference":"Nelson et al. (2019)","title":"Nudging tourists to donate for conservation: Experimental evidence on soliciting voluntary contributions for coastal management","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":275,"n_comparison":275,"n_control":146,"n_intervention":129,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.48,"mean_intervention":0.68,"cohens_d":0.4083,"variance_d":0.0149,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":154,"study_id":242,"es_id":329,"reference":"Rothman et al. (1999)","title":"The systematic influence of gain- and loss-framed messages on interest in and use of different types of health behavior","year":1999,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":60,"n_comparison":60,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":30,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.47,"mean_intervention":0.67,"cohens_d":0.407,"variance_d":0.068,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":140,"study_id":222,"es_id":301,"reference":"Patel et al. (2018)","title":"Effect of an automated patient dashboard using active choice and peer comparison performance feedback to physicians on statin prescribing: The PRESCRIBE cluster randomized clinical trial","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":64,"n_comparison":64,"n_control":32,"n_intervention":32,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2.6,"mean_intervention":6.7,"cohens_d":0.4054,"variance_d":0.0638,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":189,"study_id":296,"es_id":395,"reference":"Ubel et al. (2001)","title":"Preference for equity as a framing effect","year":2001,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":267,"n_comparison":267,"n_control":135,"n_intervention":132,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.57,"mean_intervention":0.37,"cohens_d":0.4035,"variance_d":0.0153,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":95,"study_id":159,"es_id":208,"reference":"Keller et al. (2011)","title":"Enhanced active choice: A new method to motivate behavior change","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":36,"n_comparison":36,"n_control":18,"n_intervention":18,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.42,"mean_intervention":0.62,"cohens_d":0.4031,"variance_d":0.1134,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":102,"study_id":172,"es_id":225,"reference":"Kressel et al. (2007)","title":"The influence of default options on the expression of end-of-life treatment preferences in advance directives","year":2007,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":75,"n_comparison":75,"n_control":39,"n_intervention":36,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.2,"mean_intervention":0.38,"cohens_d":0.4011,"variance_d":0.0545,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":15,"study_id":23,"es_id":27,"reference":"Basu & Savani (2017)","title":"Choosing one at a time? Presenting options simultaneously helps people make more optimal decisions than presenting options sequentially","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":214,"n_comparison":214,"n_control":107,"n_intervention":107,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":39.82,"sd_control":33.05,"mean_intervention":52.57,"sd_intervention":31.41,"cohens_d":0.3955,"variance_d":0.0191,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":29,"study_id":49,"es_id":67,"reference":"Bruns et al. (2018)","title":"Can nudges be transparent and yet effective?","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":256,"n_comparison":256,"n_control":85,"n_intervention":171,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.82,"sd_control":2.66,"mean_intervention":2.95,"sd_intervention":2.98,"cohens_d":0.3926,"variance_d":0.0179,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":32,"study_id":52,"es_id":71,"reference":"Byrd et al. (2018)","title":"Adding sodium information to casual dining restaurant menus: Beneficial or detrimental for consumers?","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":61,"n_comparison":61,"n_control":34,"n_intervention":27,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2991,"sd_control":1473.598,"mean_intervention":3544,"sd_intervention":1328.656,"cohens_d":0.3918,"variance_d":0.0677,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":175,"study_id":271,"es_id":365,"reference":"Stampfli et al. (2017)","title":"A nudge in a healthier direction: How environmental cues help restrained eaters pursue their weight-control goal","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":114,"n_comparison":114,"n_control":57,"n_intervention":57,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":21.82,"sd_control":10.81,"mean_intervention":17.83,"sd_intervention":9.68,"cohens_d":0.3889,"variance_d":0.0358,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":174,"study_id":270,"es_id":364,"reference":"Stampfli & Brunner (2016)","title":"The art of dieting: Exposure to thin sculptures effortlessly reduces the intake of unhealthy food in motivated eaters","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":128,"n_comparison":128,"n_control":64,"n_intervention":64,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":14.56,"sd_control":9.37,"mean_intervention":11.48,"sd_intervention":6.18,"cohens_d":0.3881,"variance_d":0.0318,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":95,"study_id":160,"es_id":209,"reference":"Keller et al. (2011)","title":"Enhanced active choice: A new method to motivate behavior change","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":9950,"n_comparison":9950,"n_control":5491,"n_intervention":4459,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.157,"mean_intervention":0.32,"cohens_d":0.3877,"variance_d":0.0004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":26,"study_id":46,"es_id":63,"reference":"Broman et al. (2014)","title":"The importance of framing for consumer acceptance of the smart Grid: A comparative study of Denmark, Norway, and Switzerland","year":2014,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":901,"n_comparison":901,"n_control":451,"n_intervention":450,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.58,"mean_intervention":0.76,"cohens_d":0.3862,"variance_d":0.0045,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":15,"study_id":24,"es_id":28,"reference":"Basu & Savani (2017)","title":"Choosing one at a time? Presenting options simultaneously helps people make more optimal decisions than presenting options sequentially","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":201,"n_comparison":201,"n_control":101,"n_intervention":100,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":75.46,"sd_control":25.25,"mean_intervention":84.42,"sd_intervention":21.03,"cohens_d":0.3854,"variance_d":0.0203,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":212,"study_id":334,"es_id":447,"reference":"Zuraikat et al. (2018)","title":"Doggy bags and downsizing: Packaging uneaten food to go after a meal attenuates the portion size effect in women","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":53,"n_comparison":53,"n_control":26,"n_intervention":27,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":522.7,"sd_control":149.4,"mean_intervention":473,"sd_intervention":106.52,"cohens_d":0.3843,"variance_d":0.0769,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":130,"study_id":208,"es_id":281,"reference":"Munoz et al. (2017)","title":"The impact of phone calls on follow-up rates in an online depression prevention study","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":32,"n_comparison":32,"n_control":14,"n_intervention":18,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.21427,"mean_intervention":0.38887,"cohens_d":0.3842,"variance_d":0.1293,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":160,"study_id":249,"es_id":337,"reference":"Schram & Sonnemans (2011)","title":"How individuals choose health insurance: An experimental analysis","year":2011,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":148,"n_comparison":148,"n_control":74,"n_intervention":74,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.04148,"sd_control":0.08056,"mean_intervention":0.07167,"sd_intervention":0.07834,"cohens_d":0.3799,"variance_d":0.0275,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":28,"study_id":48,"es_id":66,"reference":"Brook & Servatka (2016)","title":"The anticipatory effect of nonverbal communication","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":94,"n_comparison":62,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":32,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2.67,"sd_control":2.15,"mean_intervention":3.44,"sd_intervention":1.93,"cohens_d":0.3776,"variance_d":0.0657,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":51,"study_id":83,"es_id":114,"reference":"Dickerson et al. (1992)","title":"Using cognitive dissonanace to encourage water conservation","year":1992,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":60,"n_comparison":40,"n_control":20,"n_intervention":20,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":301.8,"sd_control":142.32,"mean_intervention":248.3,"sd_intervention":146.07,"cohens_d":0.371,"variance_d":0.1017,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":58,"study_id":99,"es_id":131,"reference":"Evans et al. (2011)","title":"Trust and self-control: The moderating role of the default","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":141,"n_comparison":141,"n_control":71,"n_intervention":70,"binary_outcome":false,"cohens_d":0.3706,"variance_d":0.0289,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":172,"study_id":267,"es_id":357,"reference":"Sonntag & Zizzo (2015)","title":"On reminder effects, drop-outs and dominance: Evidence from an online experiment on charitable giving","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":99,"n_comparison":66,"n_control":33,"n_intervention":33,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.111,"mean_intervention":0.25,"cohens_d":0.3679,"variance_d":0.0616,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":50,"study_id":82,"es_id":107,"reference":"Demarque et al. (2015)","title":"Nudging sustainable consumption: The use of descriptive norms to promote a minority behavior in a realistic online shopping environment","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":235,"n_comparison":79,"n_control":41,"n_intervention":38,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.56,"sd_control":2.16,"mean_intervention":2.29,"sd_intervention":1.84,"cohens_d":0.3627,"variance_d":0.0515,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":172,"study_id":268,"es_id":358,"reference":"Sonntag & Zizzo (2015)","title":"On reminder effects, drop-outs and dominance: Evidence from an online experiment on charitable giving","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":102,"n_comparison":68,"n_control":34,"n_intervention":34,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.091,"mean_intervention":0.219,"cohens_d":0.3611,"variance_d":0.0598,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":33,"study_id":54,"es_id":73,"reference":"Camilleri & Larrick (2014)","title":"Metric and scale design as choice architecture tools","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":424,"n_comparison":282,"n_control":141,"n_intervention":141,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.587,"mean_intervention":0.751,"cohens_d":0.351,"variance_d":0.0144,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":6,"study_id":6,"es_id":9,"reference":"Anzman-Frasca et al. (2018)","title":"Effects of a randomized intervention promoting healthy children's meals on children's ordering and dietary intake in a quick-service restaurant","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":58,"n_comparison":58,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":28,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.067,"mean_intervention":0.179,"cohens_d":0.35,"variance_d":0.0701,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":50,"study_id":81,"es_id":108,"reference":"Demarque et al. (2015)","title":"Nudging sustainable consumption: The use of descriptive norms to promote a minority behavior in a realistic online shopping environment","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":122,"n_comparison":60,"n_control":29,"n_intervention":31,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2,"sd_control":2.45,"mean_intervention":2.84,"sd_intervention":2.38,"cohens_d":0.348,"variance_d":0.0677,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":34,"study_id":56,"es_id":77,"reference":"Capraro et al. (2018)","title":"What's the right thing to do? Increasing pro-sociality with simple moral nudges","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":300,"n_comparison":300,"n_control":100,"n_intervention":200,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":21.2,"mean_intervention":30.6,"cohens_d":0.3464,"variance_d":0.0152,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":18,"study_id":31,"es_id":35,"reference":"BETA (2018)","title":"Nudge vs superbugs: A behavioural economics trial to reduce the overprescribing of antibiotics","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":3990,"n_comparison":2671,"n_control":1338,"n_intervention":1333,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":109.7,"sd_control":40.8,"mean_intervention":96.2,"sd_intervention":37.4,"cohens_d":0.3449,"variance_d":0.0015,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":59,"study_id":102,"es_id":134,"reference":"Everett et al. (2015)","title":"Doing good by doing nothing? The role of social norms in explaining default effects in altruistic context","year":2015,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":177,"n_comparison":177,"n_control":89,"n_intervention":88,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.2,"mean_intervention":0.35,"cohens_d":0.3388,"variance_d":0.0229,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":64,"study_id":111,"es_id":143,"reference":"Ganzach & Karsahi (1995)","title":"Message framing and buying behavior: A field experiment","year":1995,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":123,"n_comparison":123,"n_control":57,"n_intervention":66,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.293,"mean_intervention":0.455,"cohens_d":0.3367,"variance_d":0.0332,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":34,"study_id":57,"es_id":78,"reference":"Capraro et al. (2018)","title":"What's the right thing to do? Increasing pro-sociality with simple moral nudges","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":200,"n_comparison":200,"n_control":100,"n_intervention":100,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":18,"mean_intervention":25.5,"cohens_d":0.3345,"variance_d":0.0203,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":50,"study_id":82,"es_id":109,"reference":"Demarque et al. (2015)","title":"Nudging sustainable consumption: The use of descriptive norms to promote a minority behavior in a realistic online shopping environment","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":235,"n_comparison":81,"n_control":41,"n_intervention":40,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.56,"sd_control":2.16,"mean_intervention":2.22,"sd_intervention":1.83,"cohens_d":0.3294,"variance_d":0.0501,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":146,"study_id":231,"es_id":312,"reference":"Raue et al. (2019)","title":"The power of peers: Prompting savings behavior through social comparison","year":2019,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1155,"n_comparison":1155,"n_control":218,"n_intervention":937,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.08,"mean_intervention":0.19,"cohens_d":0.3285,"variance_d":0.0057,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":189,"study_id":297,"es_id":396,"reference":"Ubel et al. (2001)","title":"Preference for equity as a framing effect","year":2001,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":241,"n_comparison":241,"n_control":121,"n_intervention":120,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.49,"mean_intervention":0.33,"cohens_d":0.3269,"variance_d":0.0168,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":191,"study_id":299,"es_id":400,"reference":"Van Dalen & Henkens (2014)","title":"Comparing the effects of defaults on organ donation systems","year":2014,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1507,"n_comparison":994,"n_control":466,"n_intervention":528,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.5,"mean_intervention":0.66,"cohens_d":0.3257,"variance_d":0.0041,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":207,"study_id":323,"es_id":431,"reference":"Yeomans & Herberich (2014)","title":"An experimental test of the effect of negative social norms on energy-efficient investments","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":77,"n_comparison":77,"n_control":32,"n_intervention":45,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.031,"mean_intervention":0.111,"cohens_d":0.3253,"variance_d":0.0542,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":149,"study_id":236,"es_id":318,"reference":"Rigtering et al. (2019)","title":"Increasing quantity without comprising quality: How managerial framing affects intrapreneurship","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":322,"n_comparison":322,"n_control":160,"n_intervention":162,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.03125,"mean_intervention":0.1111,"cohens_d":0.3243,"variance_d":0.0126,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":59,"study_id":103,"es_id":135,"reference":"Everett et al. (2015)","title":"Doing good by doing nothing? The role of social norms in explaining default effects in altruistic context","year":2015,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":150,"n_comparison":150,"n_control":75,"n_intervention":75,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.11,"mean_intervention":0.23,"cohens_d":0.3242,"variance_d":0.027,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":150,"study_id":237,"es_id":319,"reference":"Roberto et al. (2010)","title":"Evaluating the impact of menu labeling on food choices and intake","year":2010,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":295,"n_comparison":191,"n_control":95,"n_intervention":96,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2189.37,"sd_control":1080.51,"mean_intervention":1862.23,"sd_intervention":937.29,"cohens_d":0.3236,"variance_d":0.0212,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":50,"study_id":81,"es_id":110,"reference":"Demarque et al. (2015)","title":"Nudging sustainable consumption: The use of descriptive norms to promote a minority behavior in a realistic online shopping environment","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":122,"n_comparison":59,"n_control":29,"n_intervention":30,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2,"sd_control":2.45,"mean_intervention":2.77,"sd_intervention":2.31,"cohens_d":0.3236,"variance_d":0.0687,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":124,"study_id":201,"es_id":267,"reference":"Meng & Trudel (2017)","title":"Using emoticons to encourage students to recycle","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":192,"n_comparison":192,"n_control":96,"n_intervention":96,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.46,"mean_intervention":0.62,"cohens_d":0.3225,"variance_d":0.0211,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":1,"study_id":1,"es_id":1,"reference":"Abhyankar et al. (2014)","title":"Framing options as choice or opportunity: Does the frame influence decisions?","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":124,"n_comparison":83,"n_control":42,"n_intervention":41,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.45,"mean_intervention":0.61,"cohens_d":0.322,"variance_d":0.0488,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":11,"study_id":13,"es_id":17,"reference":"Bamberg (2002)","title":"Effects of implementation intention on the actual performance of new environmentally friendly behaviours - Results of two field experiments","year":2002,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":159,"n_comparison":159,"n_control":79,"n_intervention":80,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.342,"mean_intervention":0.5,"cohens_d":0.3215,"variance_d":0.0255,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":137,"study_id":218,"es_id":297,"reference":"Or et al. (2014)","title":"Real life decision making of serious mental illness patients: Opt-in and opt-out research participation","year":2014,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":207,"n_comparison":207,"n_control":106,"n_intervention":101,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.42,"mean_intervention":0.58,"cohens_d":0.3214,"variance_d":0.0196,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":158,"study_id":246,"es_id":333,"reference":"Sass et al. (2017)","title":"Test-takers' eye movements: Effects of integration aids and types of graphical representations","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":60,"n_comparison":60,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":30,"binary_outcome":false,"cohens_d":0.3173,"variance_d":0.0675,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":188,"study_id":295,"es_id":394,"reference":"Trudel et al. (2015)","title":"The impact of traffic light color-coding on food health perceptions and choice","year":2015,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":54,"n_comparison":54,"n_control":27,"n_intervention":27,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.96,"sd_control":1.86,"mean_intervention":1.4,"sd_intervention":1.72,"cohens_d":0.3126,"variance_d":0.075,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":93,"study_id":156,"es_id":205,"reference":"Johnston et al. (2018)","title":"Just a nudge: Applying behavioral incentives to engage residents in quality improvement education","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":453,"n_comparison":453,"n_control":227,"n_intervention":226,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":44,"mean_intervention":60,"cohens_d":0.3112,"variance_d":0.0089,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":84,"study_id":139,"es_id":183,"reference":"Hilton et al. (2014)","title":"A tax can nudge: The impact of an environmentally motivated bonus/malus fiscal system on transport preferences","year":2014,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":863,"n_comparison":863,"n_control":434,"n_intervention":429,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.302,"mean_intervention":0.452,"cohens_d":0.311,"variance_d":0.0047,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":150,"study_id":237,"es_id":320,"reference":"Roberto et al. (2010)","title":"Evaluating the impact of menu labeling on food choices and intake","year":2010,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":295,"n_comparison":199,"n_control":95,"n_intervention":104,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2189.37,"sd_control":1080.51,"mean_intervention":1859.7,"sd_intervention":1062.58,"cohens_d":0.3078,"variance_d":0.0204,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":31,"study_id":51,"es_id":70,"reference":"Burns & Rothman (2015)","title":"Offering variety: A subtle manipulation to promote healthy food choice throughout the day","year":2015,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":187,"n_comparison":187,"n_control":86,"n_intervention":101,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.442,"mean_intervention":0.594,"cohens_d":0.3054,"variance_d":0.0218,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":82,"study_id":137,"es_id":180,"reference":"Hedlin & Sunstein (2016)","title":"Does active choosing promote green energy use? Experimental evidence","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1037,"n_comparison":1037,"n_control":345,"n_intervention":346,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.69,"mean_intervention":0.82,"cohens_d":0.3047,"variance_d":0.0059,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":138,"study_id":220,"es_id":299,"reference":"Paese (1995)","title":"Effects of framing on actual time allocation decisions","year":1995,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":98,"n_comparison":98,"n_control":49,"n_intervention":49,"binary_outcome":true,"cohens_d":0.3015,"variance_d":0.0422,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":133,"study_id":213,"es_id":287,"reference":"Nelson et al. (2019)","title":"Nudging tourists to donate for conservation: Experimental evidence on soliciting voluntary contributions for coastal management","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":264,"n_comparison":264,"n_control":127,"n_intervention":137,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.19,"mean_intervention":0.32,"cohens_d":0.3005,"variance_d":0.0153,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":15,"study_id":25,"es_id":29,"reference":"Basu & Savani (2017)","title":"Choosing one at a time? Presenting options simultaneously helps people make more optimal decisions than presenting options sequentially","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":293,"n_comparison":293,"n_control":147,"n_intervention":146,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":38.04,"mean_intervention":47.45,"cohens_d":0.2926,"variance_d":0.0138,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":24,"study_id":38,"es_id":55,"reference":"Bohm et al. (2016)","title":"Outcome valence and externality valence framing in public good dilemmas","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":167,"n_comparison":167,"n_control":84,"n_intervention":83,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":50.86,"sd_control":34.19,"mean_intervention":60.96,"sd_intervention":35.24,"cohens_d":0.2909,"variance_d":0.0242,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":150,"study_id":237,"es_id":321,"reference":"Roberto et al. (2010)","title":"Evaluating the impact of menu labeling on food choices and intake","year":2010,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":295,"n_comparison":199,"n_control":95,"n_intervention":104,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1458.92,"sd_control":724.62,"mean_intervention":1256.37,"sd_intervention":688.47,"cohens_d":0.2869,"variance_d":0.0203,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":61,"study_id":107,"es_id":139,"reference":"Firmino-Machado et al. (2018)","title":"Stepwise strategy to improve cervical cancer screening adherence (SCAN.Cervical Cancer) - Automated text messages, phone calls and reminders: Population based randomized controlled trial","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":1220,"n_comparison":1220,"n_control":615,"n_intervention":605,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.257,"mean_intervention":0.39,"cohens_d":0.2857,"variance_d":0.0033,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":15,"study_id":26,"es_id":30,"reference":"Basu & Savani (2017)","title":"Choosing one at a time? Presenting options simultaneously helps people make more optimal decisions than presenting options sequentially","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":634,"n_comparison":634,"n_control":317,"n_intervention":317,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":53.63,"mean_intervention":61.8,"cohens_d":0.285,"variance_d":0.0064,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":206,"study_id":321,"es_id":429,"reference":"Xue et al. (2017)","title":"Mathematics self-confidence and the \"prepayment effect\" in riskless choices","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":103,"n_comparison":103,"n_control":49,"n_intervention":54,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.735,"mean_intervention":0.602,"cohens_d":0.2838,"variance_d":0.0393,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":2,"study_id":2,"es_id":4,"reference":"Alemany et al. (2019)","title":"Enhancing the privacy risk awareness of teenagers in online social networks through soft-paternalism mechanisms","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":42,"n_comparison":30,"n_control":14,"n_intervention":16,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.457,"sd_control":0.366,"mean_intervention":0.557,"sd_intervention":0.344,"cohens_d":0.2822,"variance_d":0.1353,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":133,"study_id":214,"es_id":288,"reference":"Nelson et al. (2019)","title":"Nudging tourists to donate for conservation: Experimental evidence on soliciting voluntary contributions for coastal management","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":198,"n_comparison":198,"n_control":101,"n_intervention":97,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.45,"mean_intervention":0.59,"cohens_d":0.2812,"variance_d":0.0204,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":34,"study_id":58,"es_id":79,"reference":"Capraro et al. (2018)","title":"What's the right thing to do? Increasing pro-sociality with simple moral nudges","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":300,"n_comparison":300,"n_control":100,"n_intervention":200,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":32.9,"mean_intervention":48,"cohens_d":0.2799,"variance_d":0.0151,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":107,"study_id":179,"es_id":232,"reference":"Lehmann et al. (2016)","title":"Changing the default to promote influenza vaccination among health care workers","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":122,"n_comparison":122,"n_control":61,"n_intervention":61,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.164,"mean_intervention":0.279,"cohens_d":0.2791,"variance_d":0.0331,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":146,"study_id":232,"es_id":313,"reference":"Raue et al. (2019)","title":"The power of peers: Prompting savings behavior through social comparison","year":2019,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":630,"n_comparison":630,"n_control":197,"n_intervention":433,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.19,"mean_intervention":0.31,"cohens_d":0.2789,"variance_d":0.0074,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":121,"study_id":197,"es_id":263,"reference":"McCaul & Kopp (1982)","title":"Effects of goal setting and commitment on increasing metal recycling","year":1982,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":65,"n_comparison":65,"n_control":37,"n_intervention":28,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":27.63,"sd_control":27.17357,"mean_intervention":35.19,"sd_intervention":27.53895,"cohens_d":0.2766,"variance_d":0.0633,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":99,"study_id":168,"es_id":220,"reference":"Knowles et al. (2019)","title":"Exploring the underpinning mechanisms of the proximity effect within a competitive food environment","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":23,"n_comparison":23,"n_control":12,"n_intervention":11,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":31.33,"sd_control":35.15,"mean_intervention":41.65,"sd_intervention":41.05,"cohens_d":0.2711,"variance_d":0.1758,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":109,"study_id":181,"es_id":235,"reference":"Lieberman et al. (2019)","title":"How incentive framing can harness the power of social norms","year":2019,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":294,"n_comparison":294,"n_control":147,"n_intervention":147,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.102,"mean_intervention":0.197,"cohens_d":0.2696,"variance_d":0.0137,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":36,"study_id":63,"es_id":84,"reference":"Carroll et al. (2018)","title":"Food bundling as a health nudge: Investigating consumer fruit and vegetable selection using behavioral economics","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":122,"n_comparison":122,"n_control":61,"n_intervention":61,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":49.48,"sd_control":24.09,"mean_intervention":56.36,"sd_intervention":27.66,"cohens_d":0.2653,"variance_d":0.0331,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":171,"study_id":266,"es_id":356,"reference":"Small & Loewenstein (2003)","title":"Helping a victim or helping the victim: Altruism and identifiability","year":2003,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":234,"n_comparison":234,"n_control":116,"n_intervention":118,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2.33,"sd_control":2.31,"mean_intervention":2.93,"sd_intervention":2.25,"cohens_d":0.2632,"variance_d":0.0172,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":14,"study_id":18,"es_id":22,"reference":"Bartke et al. (2017)","title":"Social comparison nudges - Guessing the norm increases charitable giving","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":176,"n_comparison":176,"n_control":83,"n_intervention":93,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.48,"mean_intervention":0.61,"cohens_d":0.2618,"variance_d":0.023,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":173,"study_id":269,"es_id":361,"reference":"Soon et al. (2018)","title":"Effect of two behavioural 'nudging' interventions on management decisions for low back pain: A randomised vignette-based study in general practitioners","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":120,"n_comparison":60,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":30,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.425,"mean_intervention":0.3,"cohens_d":0.2609,"variance_d":0.0672,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":163,"study_id":252,"es_id":340,"reference":"Schwartz et al. (2021)","title":"Opting-in to prosocial incentives","year":2021,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":460,"n_comparison":460,"n_control":233,"n_intervention":227,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.605,"mean_intervention":0.476,"cohens_d":0.2596,"variance_d":0.0088,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":183,"study_id":290,"es_id":387,"reference":"Taylor et al. (2015)","title":"Improving social norms interventions: Rank-framing increases excessive alcohol drinkers' information-seeking","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":58,"n_comparison":38,"n_control":21,"n_intervention":17,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.048,"mean_intervention":0.118,"cohens_d":0.2595,"variance_d":0.1073,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":95,"study_id":161,"es_id":210,"reference":"Keller et al. (2011)","title":"Enhanced active choice: A new method to motivate behavior change","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":11182,"n_comparison":11182,"n_control":4232,"n_intervention":6950,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.123,"mean_intervention":0.219,"cohens_d":0.2573,"variance_d":0.0004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":211,"study_id":330,"es_id":439,"reference":"Zikmund-Fisher et al. (2011)","title":"\"I'll do what they did\": Social norm information and cancer treatment decisions.","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1119,"n_comparison":746,"n_control":373,"n_intervention":373,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.269,"mean_intervention":0.387,"cohens_d":0.2523,"variance_d":0.0054,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":125,"study_id":203,"es_id":270,"reference":"Mertens et al. (2020)","title":"This way, please: Uncovering the directional effects of attribute translations on decision making","year":2020,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":345,"n_comparison":164,"n_control":79,"n_intervention":85,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.49766,"mean_intervention":0.622,"cohens_d":0.2512,"variance_d":0.0246,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":55,"study_id":90,"es_id":121,"reference":"Dos Santos et al. (2019)","title":"Impact of nudging intervention and factors associated with vegetable dish choice among European adolescents","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":81,"n_comparison":81,"n_control":39,"n_intervention":42,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.1026,"mean_intervention":0.1905,"cohens_d":0.2512,"variance_d":0.0498,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":83,"study_id":138,"es_id":182,"reference":"Hershfield et al. (2014)","title":"National differences in environmental concern and performance are predicted by country age","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":308,"n_comparison":308,"n_control":157,"n_intervention":151,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":9.84,"sd_control":12.72,"mean_intervention":13.13,"sd_intervention":13.83,"cohens_d":0.2478,"variance_d":0.0131,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":130,"study_id":209,"es_id":282,"reference":"Munoz et al. (2017)","title":"The impact of phone calls on follow-up rates in an online depression prevention study","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":63,"n_comparison":63,"n_control":34,"n_intervention":29,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.5098,"mean_intervention":0.63213,"cohens_d":0.2478,"variance_d":0.0644,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":42,"study_id":72,"es_id":93,"reference":"Chen et al. (2016)","title":"Beware of popular kids bearing gifts: A framed field experiment","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":231,"n_comparison":231,"n_control":140,"n_intervention":91,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.19,"sd_control":1.14,"mean_intervention":1.46,"sd_intervention":1.01,"cohens_d":0.2475,"variance_d":0.0183,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":60,"study_id":104,"es_id":136,"reference":"Faralla et al. (2017)","title":"Framing effects in intertemporal choice: A nudge experiment","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":68,"n_comparison":68,"n_control":34,"n_intervention":34,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.26,"mean_intervention":0.16,"cohens_d":0.2471,"variance_d":0.0593,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":41,"study_id":71,"es_id":92,"reference":"Chapman et al. (2010)","title":"Opting in vs opting out of influenza vaccination","year":2010,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":478,"n_comparison":478,"n_control":239,"n_intervention":239,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.33,"mean_intervention":0.45,"cohens_d":0.2467,"variance_d":0.0084,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":191,"study_id":299,"es_id":401,"reference":"Van Dalen & Henkens (2014)","title":"Comparing the effects of defaults on organ donation systems","year":2014,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1507,"n_comparison":979,"n_control":466,"n_intervention":513,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.5,"mean_intervention":0.62,"cohens_d":0.2424,"variance_d":0.0041,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":7,"study_id":7,"es_id":10,"reference":"Arana & Leon (2012)","title":"Can defaults save the climate? Evidence from a field experiment on carbon offsetting programs","year":2012,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":1680,"n_comparison":1680,"n_control":856,"n_intervention":824,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.45,"mean_intervention":0.57,"cohens_d":0.2406,"variance_d":0.0024,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":173,"study_id":269,"es_id":362,"reference":"Soon et al. (2018)","title":"Effect of two behavioural 'nudging' interventions on management decisions for low back pain: A randomised vignette-based study in general practitioners","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":120,"n_comparison":59,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":29,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.425,"mean_intervention":0.31,"cohens_d":0.2392,"variance_d":0.0683,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":203,"study_id":318,"es_id":424,"reference":"Wansink et al. (2017)","title":"Larger partitions lead to larger sales: Divided grocery carts alter purchase norms and increase sales","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":83,"n_comparison":57,"n_control":26,"n_intervention":31,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":8.77,"sd_control":4.39,"mean_intervention":9.92,"sd_intervention":5.22,"cohens_d":0.2366,"variance_d":0.0712,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":36,"study_id":64,"es_id":85,"reference":"Carroll et al. (2018)","title":"Food bundling as a health nudge: Investigating consumer fruit and vegetable selection using behavioral economics","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":122,"n_comparison":122,"n_control":61,"n_intervention":61,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":48.64,"sd_control":21.77,"mean_intervention":54.5,"sd_intervention":27.58,"cohens_d":0.2359,"variance_d":0.033,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":12,"study_id":15,"es_id":19,"reference":"Banks et al. (1995)","title":"The effects of message framing on mammography utilization","year":1995,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":133,"n_comparison":133,"n_control":68,"n_intervention":65,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.338,"mean_intervention":0.453,"cohens_d":0.2358,"variance_d":0.0303,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":17,"study_id":30,"es_id":34,"reference":"BETA (2017)","title":"Effective use of SMS: Improving government confirmation processes","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":2852,"n_comparison":2852,"n_control":1437,"n_intervention":1415,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.419,"mean_intervention":0.306,"cohens_d":0.2357,"variance_d":0.0014,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":211,"study_id":331,"es_id":440,"reference":"Zikmund-Fisher et al. (2011)","title":"\"I'll do what they did\": Social norm information and cancer treatment decisions.","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1119,"n_comparison":746,"n_control":373,"n_intervention":373,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.454,"mean_intervention":0.34,"cohens_d":0.2336,"variance_d":0.0054,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":190,"study_id":298,"es_id":399,"reference":"Van Bavel et al. (2019)","title":"Using protection motivation theory in the design of nudges to improve online security behavior","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":2024,"n_comparison":1011,"n_control":507,"n_intervention":504,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":34.41,"sd_control":13.33,"mean_intervention":31.44,"sd_intervention":12.32,"cohens_d":0.2314,"variance_d":0.004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":155,"study_id":243,"es_id":330,"reference":"Ruback et al. (2014)","title":"Paying restitution: Experimental analysis of the effects of information and rationale","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":397,"n_comparison":397,"n_control":219,"n_intervention":178,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":247.647,"sd_control":797.884,"mean_intervention":651.844,"sd_intervention":2465.438,"cohens_d":0.2305,"variance_d":0.0103,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":34,"study_id":59,"es_id":80,"reference":"Capraro et al. (2018)","title":"What's the right thing to do? Increasing pro-sociality with simple moral nudges","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":900,"n_comparison":900,"n_control":450,"n_intervention":450,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":16.6,"mean_intervention":26.1,"cohens_d":0.2201,"variance_d":0.0045,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":47,"study_id":78,"es_id":100,"reference":"D'Adda et al. (2017)","title":"Push, don't nudge: Behavioral spillovers and policy instruments","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":445,"n_comparison":283,"n_control":130,"n_intervention":153,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5.338,"sd_control":5.361,"mean_intervention":6.458,"sd_intervention":5.018,"cohens_d":0.2163,"variance_d":0.0143,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":15,"study_id":27,"es_id":31,"reference":"Basu & Savani (2017)","title":"Choosing one at a time? Presenting options simultaneously helps people make more optimal decisions than presenting options sequentially","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":472,"n_comparison":472,"n_control":236,"n_intervention":236,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":54.87,"sd_control":28.84,"mean_intervention":61.34,"sd_intervention":31.42,"cohens_d":0.2145,"variance_d":0.0085,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":114,"study_id":189,"es_id":245,"reference":"Maas et al. (2011)","title":"Do distant foods decrease intake? The effect of food accessibility on consumption","year":2011,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":54,"n_comparison":36,"n_control":19,"n_intervention":17,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":12.53,"sd_control":11.49,"mean_intervention":9.53,"sd_intervention":16.5,"cohens_d":0.2132,"variance_d":0.1121,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":97,"study_id":165,"es_id":216,"reference":"Kesternich et al. (2019)","title":"The power of active choice: Field experimental evidence on repeated contribution decisions to a carbon offsetting program","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":9733,"n_comparison":9733,"n_control":4838,"n_intervention":4895,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.177,"mean_intervention":0.265,"cohens_d":0.213,"variance_d":0.0004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":134,"study_id":215,"es_id":289,"reference":"Niven et al. (2019)","title":"Effects of health star labelling on the healthiness of adults' fast food meal selections: An experimental study","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1007,"n_comparison":503,"n_control":252,"n_intervention":251,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":3.68,"sd_control":3.88764,"mean_intervention":2.88,"sd_intervention":3.63742,"cohens_d":0.2125,"variance_d":0.008,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":184,"study_id":291,"es_id":389,"reference":"Theotokis & Manganari (2015)","title":"The impact of choice architecture on consumer behavior: The role of guilt","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":107,"n_comparison":71,"n_control":35,"n_intervention":36,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.7,"mean_intervention":0.79,"cohens_d":0.2072,"variance_d":0.0567,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":211,"study_id":332,"es_id":441,"reference":"Zikmund-Fisher et al. (2011)","title":"\"I'll do what they did\": Social norm information and cancer treatment decisions.","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1077,"n_comparison":718,"n_control":359,"n_intervention":359,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.266,"mean_intervention":0.359,"cohens_d":0.2011,"variance_d":0.0056,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":164,"study_id":257,"es_id":346,"reference":"Sharif & Shu (2021)","title":"Nudging persistence after failure through emergency reserves","year":2021,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":797,"n_comparison":797,"n_control":392,"n_intervention":405,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.51,"sd_control":1.22,"mean_intervention":1.76,"sd_intervention":1.34,"cohens_d":0.1949,"variance_d":0.005,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":33,"study_id":55,"es_id":74,"reference":"Camilleri & Larrick (2014)","title":"Metric and scale design as choice architecture tools","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":484,"n_comparison":322,"n_control":161,"n_intervention":161,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.473,"mean_intervention":0.57,"cohens_d":0.1945,"variance_d":0.0125,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":108,"study_id":180,"es_id":234,"reference":"Libotte et al. (2014)","title":"The influence of plate size on meal composition. Literature review and experiment","year":2014,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":83,"n_comparison":83,"n_control":42,"n_intervention":41,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":3644,"sd_control":1376,"mean_intervention":3940,"sd_intervention":1665,"cohens_d":0.194,"variance_d":0.0484,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":114,"study_id":189,"es_id":246,"reference":"Maas et al. (2011)","title":"Do distant foods decrease intake? The effect of food accessibility on consumption","year":2011,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":54,"n_comparison":37,"n_control":19,"n_intervention":18,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":12.53,"sd_control":11.49,"mean_intervention":9.94,"sd_intervention":16.09,"cohens_d":0.1861,"variance_d":0.1087,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":60,"study_id":105,"es_id":137,"reference":"Faralla et al. (2017)","title":"Framing effects in intertemporal choice: A nudge experiment","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":241,"n_comparison":241,"n_control":121,"n_intervention":120,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.43,"mean_intervention":0.34,"cohens_d":0.1853,"variance_d":0.0167,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":150,"study_id":237,"es_id":322,"reference":"Roberto et al. (2010)","title":"Evaluating the impact of menu labeling on food choices and intake","year":2010,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":295,"n_comparison":191,"n_control":95,"n_intervention":96,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1458.92,"sd_control":724.62,"mean_intervention":1334.72,"sd_intervention":620.65,"cohens_d":0.1842,"variance_d":0.021,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":145,"study_id":229,"es_id":309,"reference":"Putnam-Farr & Riis (2016)","title":"\"Yes/no/not right now\": Yes/no response formats can increase response rates even in non-forced-choice settings","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":14939,"n_comparison":14939,"n_control":6377,"n_intervention":8562,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.032,"mean_intervention":0.072,"cohens_d":0.1836,"variance_d":0.0003,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":33,"study_id":54,"es_id":75,"reference":"Camilleri & Larrick (2014)","title":"Metric and scale design as choice architecture tools","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":424,"n_comparison":283,"n_control":142,"n_intervention":141,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.672,"mean_intervention":0.751,"cohens_d":0.1747,"variance_d":0.0142,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":60,"study_id":106,"es_id":138,"reference":"Faralla et al. (2017)","title":"Framing effects in intertemporal choice: A nudge experiment","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":135,"n_comparison":135,"n_control":68,"n_intervention":67,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.24,"mean_intervention":0.17,"cohens_d":0.174,"variance_d":0.0297,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":164,"study_id":258,"es_id":347,"reference":"Sharif & Shu (2021)","title":"Nudging persistence after failure through emergency reserves","year":2021,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":600,"n_comparison":600,"n_control":302,"n_intervention":298,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.652,"mean_intervention":0.732,"cohens_d":0.1736,"variance_d":0.0067,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":70,"study_id":122,"es_id":157,"reference":"Gomez et al. (2016)","title":"The attraction effect in mid-involvement categories: An experimental economics approach","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":100,"n_comparison":100,"n_control":50,"n_intervention":50,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.53543,"mean_intervention":0.62069,"cohens_d":0.1729,"variance_d":0.0401,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":144,"study_id":228,"es_id":308,"reference":"Pugatch & Wilson (2018)","title":"Nudging study habits: A field experiment on peer tutoring in higher education","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":639,"n_comparison":639,"n_control":327,"n_intervention":312,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.29,"mean_intervention":0.37,"cohens_d":0.1704,"variance_d":0.0063,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":211,"study_id":331,"es_id":442,"reference":"Zikmund-Fisher et al. (2011)","title":"\"I'll do what they did\": Social norm information and cancer treatment decisions.","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1119,"n_comparison":746,"n_control":373,"n_intervention":373,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.454,"mean_intervention":0.539,"cohens_d":0.1702,"variance_d":0.0054,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":196,"study_id":305,"es_id":408,"reference":"Veldwijk et al. (2016)","title":"Survival or mortality: Does risk attribute framing influence decision-making behavior in a discrete choice experiment?","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1045,"n_comparison":1045,"n_control":513,"n_intervention":532,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.049,"mean_intervention":0.092,"cohens_d":0.1699,"variance_d":0.0038,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":44,"study_id":75,"es_id":96,"reference":"Chou & Murnighan (2013)","title":"Life or death decisions: Framing the call for help","year":2013,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":182,"n_comparison":182,"n_control":91,"n_intervention":91,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.521,"mean_intervention":0.602,"cohens_d":0.1634,"variance_d":0.0221,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":82,"study_id":137,"es_id":181,"reference":"Hedlin & Sunstein (2016)","title":"Does active choosing promote green energy use? Experimental evidence","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1037,"n_comparison":1037,"n_control":345,"n_intervention":346,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.69,"mean_intervention":0.76,"cohens_d":0.1571,"variance_d":0.0058,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":159,"study_id":248,"es_id":336,"reference":"Saulais et al. (2016)","title":"When are \"Dish of the Day\" nudges most effective to increase vegetable selection?","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":173,"n_comparison":116,"n_control":61,"n_intervention":55,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.656,"mean_intervention":0.727,"cohens_d":0.154,"variance_d":0.0347,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":112,"study_id":185,"es_id":239,"reference":"Lofgren et al. (2012)","title":"Are experienced people affected by a pre-set default option: Results from a field experiment","year":2012,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":240,"n_comparison":160,"n_control":80,"n_intervention":80,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.393,"mean_intervention":0.468,"cohens_d":0.1516,"variance_d":0.0251,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":38,"study_id":66,"es_id":87,"reference":"Castleman & Page (2015)","title":"Summer nudging: Can personalized text messages and peer mentor outreach increase college going among low-income high school graduates?","year":2015,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":746,"n_comparison":746,"n_control":373,"n_intervention":373,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.628,"mean_intervention":0.699,"cohens_d":0.1505,"variance_d":0.0054,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":145,"study_id":230,"es_id":310,"reference":"Putnam-Farr & Riis (2016)","title":"\"Yes/no/not right now\": Yes/no response formats can increase response rates even in non-forced-choice settings","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":24863,"n_comparison":15987,"n_control":8227,"n_intervention":7760,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.087,"mean_intervention":0.133,"cohens_d":0.1478,"variance_d":0.0003,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":181,"study_id":288,"es_id":383,"reference":"Tasoff & Letzler (2014)","title":"Everyone believes in redemption: Nudges and overoptimism in costly task completion","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":310,"n_comparison":197,"n_control":139,"n_intervention":58,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.31,"mean_intervention":0.38,"cohens_d":0.1474,"variance_d":0.0245,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":73,"study_id":126,"es_id":164,"reference":"Goswami & Urminsky (2016)","title":"When should the ask be a nudge? The effect of default amounts on charitable donations","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":105,"n_comparison":70,"n_control":35,"n_intervention":35,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.03,"mean_intervention":0.06,"cohens_d":0.1468,"variance_d":0.0573,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":34,"study_id":60,"es_id":81,"reference":"Capraro et al. (2018)","title":"What's the right thing to do? Increasing pro-sociality with simple moral nudges","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":900,"n_comparison":900,"n_control":450,"n_intervention":450,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":11.7,"mean_intervention":17.9,"cohens_d":0.1458,"variance_d":0.0045,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":95,"study_id":158,"es_id":211,"reference":"Keller et al. (2011)","title":"Enhanced active choice: A new method to motivate behavior change","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":110,"n_comparison":56,"n_control":28,"n_intervention":28,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.45,"mean_intervention":0.52,"cohens_d":0.1402,"variance_d":0.0716,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":95,"study_id":158,"es_id":212,"reference":"Keller et al. (2011)","title":"Enhanced active choice: A new method to motivate behavior change","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":110,"n_comparison":55,"n_control":28,"n_intervention":27,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.45,"mean_intervention":0.52,"cohens_d":0.1402,"variance_d":0.0729,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":125,"study_id":202,"es_id":271,"reference":"Mertens et al. (2020)","title":"This way, please: Uncovering the directional effects of attribute translations on decision making","year":2020,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":356,"n_comparison":188,"n_control":104,"n_intervention":84,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.50537,"mean_intervention":0.5727,"cohens_d":0.1352,"variance_d":0.0216,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":172,"study_id":267,"es_id":359,"reference":"Sonntag & Zizzo (2015)","title":"On reminder effects, drop-outs and dominance: Evidence from an online experiment on charitable giving","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":99,"n_comparison":66,"n_control":33,"n_intervention":33,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.111,"mean_intervention":0.156,"cohens_d":0.1327,"variance_d":0.0607,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":73,"study_id":127,"es_id":165,"reference":"Goswami & Urminsky (2016)","title":"When should the ask be a nudge? The effect of default amounts on charitable donations","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":3486,"n_comparison":3486,"n_control":819,"n_intervention":2667,"binary_outcome":true,"cohens_d":0.1316,"variance_d":0.0016,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":11,"study_id":14,"es_id":18,"reference":"Bamberg (2002)","title":"Effects of implementation intention on the actual performance of new environmentally friendly behaviours - Results of two field experiments","year":2002,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":160,"n_comparison":160,"n_control":81,"n_intervention":79,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.543,"mean_intervention":0.608,"cohens_d":0.1316,"variance_d":0.0251,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":134,"study_id":215,"es_id":290,"reference":"Niven et al. (2019)","title":"Effects of health star labelling on the healthiness of adults' fast food meal selections: An experimental study","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1007,"n_comparison":503,"n_control":252,"n_intervention":251,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":3.68,"sd_control":3.88764,"mean_intervention":3.17,"sd_intervention":3.87991,"cohens_d":0.1313,"variance_d":0.008,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":125,"study_id":203,"es_id":272,"reference":"Mertens et al. (2020)","title":"This way, please: Uncovering the directional effects of attribute translations on decision making","year":2020,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":345,"n_comparison":171,"n_control":79,"n_intervention":92,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.49766,"mean_intervention":0.56219,"cohens_d":0.1294,"variance_d":0.0236,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":55,"study_id":91,"es_id":122,"reference":"Dos Santos et al. (2019)","title":"Impact of nudging intervention and factors associated with vegetable dish choice among European adolescents","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":113,"n_comparison":113,"n_control":53,"n_intervention":60,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.0943,"mean_intervention":0.1333,"cohens_d":0.1232,"variance_d":0.0356,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":1,"study_id":1,"es_id":2,"reference":"Abhyankar et al. (2014)","title":"Framing options as choice or opportunity: Does the frame influence decisions?","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":124,"n_comparison":83,"n_control":41,"n_intervention":42,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.39,"mean_intervention":0.45,"cohens_d":0.1216,"variance_d":0.0483,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":164,"study_id":257,"es_id":348,"reference":"Sharif & Shu (2021)","title":"Nudging persistence after failure through emergency reserves","year":2021,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":797,"n_comparison":797,"n_control":392,"n_intervention":405,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.83,"sd_control":1.58,"mean_intervention":2.03,"sd_intervention":1.73,"cohens_d":0.1206,"variance_d":0.005,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":209,"study_id":328,"es_id":436,"reference":"Zarghamee et al. (2017)","title":"Nudging charitable giving: Three field experiments","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":70,"n_comparison":70,"n_control":36,"n_intervention":34,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":4.28,"sd_control":9.13,"mean_intervention":5.32,"sd_intervention":8.76,"cohens_d":0.1162,"variance_d":0.0573,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":134,"study_id":215,"es_id":291,"reference":"Niven et al. (2019)","title":"Effects of health star labelling on the healthiness of adults' fast food meal selections: An experimental study","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1007,"n_comparison":503,"n_control":252,"n_intervention":251,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5452,"sd_control":2790.188,"mean_intervention":5124,"sd_intervention":2934.184,"cohens_d":0.1146,"variance_d":0.008,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":163,"study_id":253,"es_id":341,"reference":"Schwartz et al. (2021)","title":"Opting-in to prosocial incentives","year":2021,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":437,"n_comparison":437,"n_control":216,"n_intervention":221,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.236,"mean_intervention":0.19,"cohens_d":0.1125,"variance_d":0.0092,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":50,"study_id":82,"es_id":111,"reference":"Demarque et al. (2015)","title":"Nudging sustainable consumption: The use of descriptive norms to promote a minority behavior in a realistic online shopping environment","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":235,"n_comparison":81,"n_control":41,"n_intervention":40,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.56,"sd_control":2.16,"mean_intervention":1.77,"sd_intervention":1.54,"cohens_d":0.1117,"variance_d":0.0495,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":172,"study_id":268,"es_id":360,"reference":"Sonntag & Zizzo (2015)","title":"On reminder effects, drop-outs and dominance: Evidence from an online experiment on charitable giving","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":102,"n_comparison":68,"n_control":34,"n_intervention":34,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.091,"mean_intervention":0.125,"cohens_d":0.1099,"variance_d":0.0589,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":163,"study_id":254,"es_id":342,"reference":"Schwartz et al. (2021)","title":"Opting-in to prosocial incentives","year":2021,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1266,"n_comparison":1266,"n_control":629,"n_intervention":637,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.49,"mean_intervention":0.436,"cohens_d":0.1084,"variance_d":0.0032,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":119,"study_id":195,"es_id":259,"reference":"Martins & Szrek (2019)","title":"The impact of the decision environment on consumer choice of mobile service plans: An experimental examination","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":355,"n_comparison":355,"n_control":178,"n_intervention":177,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":7.56,"sd_control":8.48,"mean_intervention":6.69,"sd_intervention":7.59,"cohens_d":0.1081,"variance_d":0.0113,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":18,"study_id":31,"es_id":36,"reference":"BETA (2018)","title":"Nudge vs superbugs: A behavioural economics trial to reduce the overprescribing of antibiotics","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":3990,"n_comparison":2657,"n_control":1338,"n_intervention":1319,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":109.7,"sd_control":40.8,"mean_intervention":105.5,"sd_intervention":37.7,"cohens_d":0.1069,"variance_d":0.0015,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":118,"study_id":194,"es_id":258,"reference":"Martin & Norton (2009)","title":"Shaping online consumer choice by partitioning the web","year":2009,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":124,"n_comparison":83,"n_control":42,"n_intervention":41,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.84,"mean_intervention":0.8,"cohens_d":0.1043,"variance_d":0.0483,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":178,"study_id":281,"es_id":375,"reference":"Stikvoort et al. (2016)","title":"Thou shalt not sell nature: How taboo trade-offs can make us act pro-environmentally, to clear our conscience","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":138,"n_comparison":138,"n_control":69,"n_intervention":69,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.36,"mean_intervention":0.41,"cohens_d":0.1028,"variance_d":0.029,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":89,"study_id":148,"es_id":197,"reference":"Jin (2011)","title":"Improving response rates in web surveys with default setting: The effects of default on web survey participation and permission","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":112,"n_comparison":112,"n_control":56,"n_intervention":56,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.08,"mean_intervention":0.11,"cohens_d":0.1026,"variance_d":0.0358,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":88,"study_id":145,"es_id":194,"reference":"Isaksen et al. (2019)","title":"Positive framing does not solve the tragedy of the commons","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":48,"n_comparison":48,"n_control":32,"n_intervention":16,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.18,"sd_control":0.34,"mean_intervention":0.21,"sd_intervention":0.16,"cohens_d":0.1021,"variance_d":0.0939,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":115,"study_id":190,"es_id":247,"reference":"Mann & Bryant (2019)","title":"If you ask, they will come (to register and vote): Field experiments with state election agencies on encouraging voter registration","year":2019,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":30247,"n_comparison":12074,"n_control":6028,"n_intervention":6046,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.068,"mean_intervention":0.094,"cohens_d":0.0956,"variance_d":0.0003,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":113,"study_id":186,"es_id":241,"reference":"Loibl et al. (2018)","title":"Testing strategies to increase saving in individual development account programs","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":302,"n_comparison":302,"n_control":123,"n_intervention":179,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.683,"mean_intervention":0.7265,"cohens_d":0.0954,"variance_d":0.0137,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":115,"study_id":191,"es_id":248,"reference":"Mann & Bryant (2019)","title":"If you ask, they will come (to register and vote): Field experiments with state election agencies on encouraging voter registration","year":2019,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":769686,"n_comparison":214960,"n_control":30439,"n_intervention":184521,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.046,"mean_intervention":0.068,"cohens_d":0.0953,"variance_d":0.00004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":115,"study_id":191,"es_id":249,"reference":"Mann & Bryant (2019)","title":"If you ask, they will come (to register and vote): Field experiments with state election agencies on encouraging voter registration","year":2019,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":769686,"n_comparison":215183,"n_control":30439,"n_intervention":184744,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.046,"mean_intervention":0.068,"cohens_d":0.0953,"variance_d":0.00004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":115,"study_id":191,"es_id":250,"reference":"Mann & Bryant (2019)","title":"If you ask, they will come (to register and vote): Field experiments with state election agencies on encouraging voter registration","year":2019,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":769686,"n_comparison":215371,"n_control":30439,"n_intervention":184932,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.046,"mean_intervention":0.068,"cohens_d":0.0953,"variance_d":0.00004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":145,"study_id":230,"es_id":311,"reference":"Putnam-Farr & Riis (2016)","title":"\"Yes/no/not right now\": Yes/no response formats can increase response rates even in non-forced-choice settings","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":24863,"n_comparison":16636,"n_control":8876,"n_intervention":7760,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.103,"mean_intervention":0.133,"cohens_d":0.0932,"variance_d":0.0002,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":156,"study_id":244,"es_id":331,"reference":"Sacarny et al. (2018)","title":"Effect of peer comparison letters for high-volume primary care prescribers of quetiapine in older and disabled adults: A randomized clinical trial","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":5055,"n_comparison":5055,"n_control":2528,"n_intervention":2527,"binary_outcome":false,"cohens_d":0.0926,"variance_d":0.0008,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":128,"study_id":206,"es_id":278,"reference":"Miller et al. (2016)","title":"The effects of pre-ordering and behavioral nudges on national school lunch program participants' food item selection","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":133,"n_comparison":133,"n_control":98,"n_intervention":35,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.711,"mean_intervention":0.752,"cohens_d":0.0926,"variance_d":0.0388,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":48,"study_id":79,"es_id":102,"reference":"Damgaard & Gravert (2017)","title":"The hidden costs of nudging: Experimental evidence from reminders in fundraising","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":17391,"n_comparison":17391,"n_control":8692,"n_intervention":8699,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.0214,"mean_intervention":0.0367,"cohens_d":0.0919,"variance_d":0.0002,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":115,"study_id":191,"es_id":251,"reference":"Mann & Bryant (2019)","title":"If you ask, they will come (to register and vote): Field experiments with state election agencies on encouraging voter registration","year":2019,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":769686,"n_comparison":215489,"n_control":30439,"n_intervention":185050,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.046,"mean_intervention":0.067,"cohens_d":0.0913,"variance_d":0.00004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":136,"study_id":217,"es_id":296,"reference":"O'Leary et al. (2015)","title":"Effectiveness and cost of bidirectional text messaging for adolescent vaccines and well care","year":2015,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":4587,"n_comparison":4587,"n_control":2359,"n_intervention":2228,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.103,"mean_intervention":0.132,"cohens_d":0.0902,"variance_d":0.0009,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":115,"study_id":190,"es_id":252,"reference":"Mann & Bryant (2019)","title":"If you ask, they will come (to register and vote): Field experiments with state election agencies on encouraging voter registration","year":2019,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":30247,"n_comparison":12073,"n_control":6028,"n_intervention":6045,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.068,"mean_intervention":0.092,"cohens_d":0.0887,"variance_d":0.0003,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":186,"study_id":293,"es_id":391,"reference":"Thunstrom et al. (2018)","title":"Nudges that hurt those already hurting: Distributional and unintended effects of salience nudges","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":410,"n_comparison":341,"n_control":165,"n_intervention":176,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2.189,"sd_control":2.195,"mean_intervention":1.99,"sd_intervention":2.317,"cohens_d":0.0881,"variance_d":0.0118,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":185,"study_id":292,"es_id":390,"reference":"Thorndike et al. (2016)","title":"Social norms and financial incentives to promote employees' healthy food choices: A randomized controlled trial","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":1711,"n_comparison":1711,"n_control":858,"n_intervention":853,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":0.1,"mean_intervention":1.8,"cohens_d":0.0877,"variance_d":0.0023,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":86,"study_id":141,"es_id":185,"reference":"Hu et al. (2018)","title":"Engaging scientists in science communication: The effect of social proof and meaning","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":2946,"n_comparison":2946,"n_control":1454,"n_intervention":1492,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.01032,"mean_intervention":0.02078,"cohens_d":0.0858,"variance_d":0.0014,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":181,"study_id":288,"es_id":384,"reference":"Tasoff & Letzler (2014)","title":"Everyone believes in redemption: Nudges and overoptimism in costly task completion","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":310,"n_comparison":193,"n_control":139,"n_intervention":54,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.31,"mean_intervention":0.35,"cohens_d":0.0851,"variance_d":0.0257,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":127,"study_id":205,"es_id":275,"reference":"Milkman et al. (2011)","title":"Using implementation intentions prompts to enhance influenza vaccination rates","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":3272,"n_comparison":2538,"n_control":1268,"n_intervention":1270,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.331,"mean_intervention":0.371,"cohens_d":0.0838,"variance_d":0.0016,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":100,"study_id":169,"es_id":221,"reference":"Korn et al. (2018)","title":"Social nudging: The effect of social feedback interventions on vaccine uptake","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":288,"n_comparison":288,"n_control":144,"n_intervention":144,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.49,"mean_intervention":0.53,"cohens_d":0.08,"variance_d":0.0139,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":110,"study_id":182,"es_id":236,"reference":"Liu et al. (2016)","title":"The dark side of positive social influence","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":3677,"n_comparison":3677,"n_control":1910,"n_intervention":1767,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.058,"mean_intervention":0.078,"cohens_d":0.0797,"variance_d":0.0011,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":112,"study_id":185,"es_id":240,"reference":"Lofgren et al. (2012)","title":"Are experienced people affected by a pre-set default option: Results from a field experiment","year":2012,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":240,"n_comparison":160,"n_control":80,"n_intervention":80,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.393,"mean_intervention":0.432,"cohens_d":0.0792,"variance_d":0.025,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":13,"study_id":16,"es_id":20,"reference":"Barnes et al. (2021)","title":"More intelligent designs: Comparing the effectiveness of choice architectures in US health insurance marketplaces","year":2021,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":2149,"n_comparison":2149,"n_control":1072,"n_intervention":1077,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.325,"mean_intervention":0.361,"cohens_d":0.0759,"variance_d":0.0019,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":115,"study_id":190,"es_id":253,"reference":"Mann & Bryant (2019)","title":"If you ask, they will come (to register and vote): Field experiments with state election agencies on encouraging voter registration","year":2019,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":30247,"n_comparison":12108,"n_control":6028,"n_intervention":6080,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.068,"mean_intervention":0.086,"cohens_d":0.0676,"variance_d":0.0003,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":115,"study_id":190,"es_id":254,"reference":"Mann & Bryant (2019)","title":"If you ask, they will come (to register and vote): Field experiments with state election agencies on encouraging voter registration","year":2019,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":30247,"n_comparison":12076,"n_control":6028,"n_intervention":6048,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.068,"mean_intervention":0.086,"cohens_d":0.0676,"variance_d":0.0003,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":94,"study_id":157,"es_id":206,"reference":"Junger et al. (2017)","title":"Priming and warnings are not effective to prevent social engineering attacks","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":184,"n_comparison":184,"n_control":96,"n_intervention":88,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.76726,"mean_intervention":0.73833,"cohens_d":0.0671,"variance_d":0.0218,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":20,"study_id":33,"es_id":39,"reference":"BETA (2019)","title":"Credit when it's due","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":16030,"n_comparison":8009,"n_control":4001,"n_intervention":4008,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":551,"sd_control":1759,"mean_intervention":673,"sd_intervention":2109,"cohens_d":0.0628,"variance_d":0.0005,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":21,"study_id":34,"es_id":42,"reference":"Bhanot (2017)","title":"Cheap promises: Evidence from loan repayment pledges in an online experiment","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":2787,"n_comparison":1417,"n_control":729,"n_intervention":688,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.56,"mean_intervention":0.591,"cohens_d":0.0627,"variance_d":0.0028,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":55,"study_id":92,"es_id":123,"reference":"Dos Santos et al. (2019)","title":"Impact of nudging intervention and factors associated with vegetable dish choice among European adolescents","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":84,"n_comparison":84,"n_control":42,"n_intervention":42,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.1667,"mean_intervention":0.1905,"cohens_d":0.0622,"variance_d":0.0476,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":21,"study_id":34,"es_id":43,"reference":"Bhanot (2017)","title":"Cheap promises: Evidence from loan repayment pledges in an online experiment","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":2787,"n_comparison":1417,"n_control":729,"n_intervention":688,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.78,"mean_intervention":0.805,"cohens_d":0.0617,"variance_d":0.0028,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":34,"study_id":61,"es_id":82,"reference":"Capraro et al. (2018)","title":"What's the right thing to do? Increasing pro-sociality with simple moral nudges","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":600,"n_comparison":600,"n_control":300,"n_intervention":300,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":30.9,"sd_control":110.3864,"mean_intervention":36.8,"sd_intervention":79.05611,"cohens_d":0.0615,"variance_d":0.0067,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":77,"study_id":131,"es_id":174,"reference":"Hallsworth et al. (2015)","title":"Stating appointment costs in SMS reminders reduces missed hospital appointments: Findings from two randomised controlled trials","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":5014,"n_comparison":5014,"n_control":2461,"n_intervention":2553,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.082,"mean_intervention":0.099,"cohens_d":0.0593,"variance_d":0.0008,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":47,"study_id":78,"es_id":101,"reference":"D'Adda et al. (2017)","title":"Push, don't nudge: Behavioral spillovers and policy instruments","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":445,"n_comparison":292,"n_control":130,"n_intervention":162,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5.338,"sd_control":5.361,"mean_intervention":5.642,"sd_intervention":4.944,"cohens_d":0.0592,"variance_d":0.0139,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":49,"study_id":80,"es_id":104,"reference":"De Wild et al. (2015)","title":"Influence of choice on vegetable intake in chrildren: An in-home study","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":70,"n_comparison":70,"n_control":36,"n_intervention":34,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":52.1,"sd_control":35,"mean_intervention":54.2,"sd_intervention":36,"cohens_d":0.0592,"variance_d":0.0572,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":72,"study_id":124,"es_id":159,"reference":"Gopalan et al. (2014)","title":"Translating the hemoglobin A1C with more easily understood feedback: A randomized controlled trial","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":177,"n_comparison":119,"n_control":58,"n_intervention":61,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":-0.74,"sd_control":2.81784,"mean_intervention":-0.89,"sd_intervention":2.34307,"cohens_d":0.058,"variance_d":0.0336,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":125,"study_id":202,"es_id":273,"reference":"Mertens et al. (2020)","title":"This way, please: Uncovering the directional effects of attribute translations on decision making","year":2020,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":356,"n_comparison":183,"n_control":104,"n_intervention":79,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.50537,"mean_intervention":0.53428,"cohens_d":0.0579,"variance_d":0.0223,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":9,"study_id":10,"es_id":13,"reference":"Bacon & Krpan (2018)","title":"(Not) eating for the environment: The impact of restaurant menu design on vegetarian food choice","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":565,"n_comparison":379,"n_control":194,"n_intervention":185,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.13,"mean_intervention":0.15,"cohens_d":0.0577,"variance_d":0.0106,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":211,"study_id":333,"es_id":443,"reference":"Zikmund-Fisher et al. (2011)","title":"\"I'll do what they did\": Social norm information and cancer treatment decisions.","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1077,"n_comparison":718,"n_control":359,"n_intervention":359,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.461,"mean_intervention":0.489,"cohens_d":0.0561,"variance_d":0.0056,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":193,"study_id":301,"es_id":404,"reference":"Van Kleef et al. (2012)","title":"Healthy snacks at the checkout counter: A lab and field study on the impact of shelf arrangement and assortment structure on consumer choice","year":2012,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":158,"n_comparison":158,"n_control":79,"n_intervention":79,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.2785,"mean_intervention":0.3038,"cohens_d":0.0557,"variance_d":0.0253,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":38,"study_id":67,"es_id":88,"reference":"Castleman & Page (2015)","title":"Summer nudging: Can personalized text messages and peer mentor outreach increase college going among low-income high school graduates?","year":2015,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":2920,"n_comparison":2920,"n_control":1466,"n_intervention":1454,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.718,"mean_intervention":0.742,"cohens_d":0.0541,"variance_d":0.0014,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":50,"study_id":82,"es_id":112,"reference":"Demarque et al. (2015)","title":"Nudging sustainable consumption: The use of descriptive norms to promote a minority behavior in a realistic online shopping environment","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":235,"n_comparison":80,"n_control":41,"n_intervention":39,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.56,"sd_control":2.16,"mean_intervention":1.67,"sd_intervention":1.9,"cohens_d":0.054,"variance_d":0.05,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":127,"study_id":205,"es_id":276,"reference":"Milkman et al. (2011)","title":"Using implementation intentions prompts to enhance influenza vaccination rates","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":3272,"n_comparison":2002,"n_control":1268,"n_intervention":734,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.331,"mean_intervention":0.356,"cohens_d":0.0527,"variance_d":0.0022,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":134,"study_id":215,"es_id":292,"reference":"Niven et al. (2019)","title":"Effects of health star labelling on the healthiness of adults' fast food meal selections: An experimental study","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1007,"n_comparison":505,"n_control":252,"n_intervention":253,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5452,"sd_control":2790.188,"mean_intervention":5306,"sd_intervention":2982.37,"cohens_d":0.0506,"variance_d":0.0079,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":105,"study_id":176,"es_id":229,"reference":"Lalor & Hailey (1989)","title":"The effects of message framing and feelings of susceptibility to breast cancer on reported frequency of breast self-examination","year":1989,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":24,"n_comparison":24,"n_control":12,"n_intervention":12,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":1.92,"sd_control":1.24,"mean_intervention":2,"sd_intervention":1.86,"cohens_d":0.0506,"variance_d":0.1667,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":21,"study_id":34,"es_id":44,"reference":"Bhanot (2017)","title":"Cheap promises: Evidence from loan repayment pledges in an online experiment","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":2787,"n_comparison":1424,"n_control":729,"n_intervention":695,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.56,"mean_intervention":0.585,"cohens_d":0.0505,"variance_d":0.0028,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":22,"study_id":35,"es_id":48,"reference":"BIT (2013)","title":"Applying behavioural insights to organ donation: Preliminary results from a randomised controlled trial","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":813990,"n_comparison":271330,"n_control":135665,"n_intervention":135665,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.02308,"mean_intervention":0.03125,"cohens_d":0.0504,"variance_d":0.00001,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":21,"study_id":34,"es_id":45,"reference":"Bhanot (2017)","title":"Cheap promises: Evidence from loan repayment pledges in an online experiment","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":2787,"n_comparison":1404,"n_control":729,"n_intervention":675,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.78,"mean_intervention":0.8,"cohens_d":0.0491,"variance_d":0.0029,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":46,"study_id":77,"es_id":99,"reference":"Coventry et al. (2016)","title":"Personality and social framing in privacy decision-making: A study on cookie acceptance","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":290,"n_comparison":182,"n_control":88,"n_intervention":94,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.78,"mean_intervention":0.8,"cohens_d":0.0491,"variance_d":0.022,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":13,"study_id":17,"es_id":21,"reference":"Barnes et al. (2021)","title":"More intelligent designs: Comparing the effectiveness of choice architectures in US health insurance marketplaces","year":2021,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":2397,"n_comparison":2397,"n_control":1208,"n_intervention":1189,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.465,"mean_intervention":0.489,"cohens_d":0.0481,"variance_d":0.0017,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":73,"study_id":127,"es_id":166,"reference":"Goswami & Urminsky (2016)","title":"When should the ask be a nudge? The effect of default amounts on charitable donations","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":3486,"n_comparison":3486,"n_control":819,"n_intervention":2667,"binary_outcome":true,"cohens_d":0.0451,"variance_d":0.0016,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":151,"study_id":238,"es_id":323,"reference":"Rodriguez & Saavedra (2019)","title":"The persistent effects of youth savings reminders: Experimental evidence from text-message campaigns in Colombia","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":7795,"n_comparison":5375,"n_control":2780,"n_intervention":2595,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":11.9196,"sd_control":519.8261,"mean_intervention":30.39553,"sd_intervention":291.8809,"cohens_d":0.0434,"variance_d":0.0007,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":119,"study_id":195,"es_id":260,"reference":"Martins & Szrek (2019)","title":"The impact of the decision environment on consumer choice of mobile service plans: An experimental examination","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":355,"n_comparison":355,"n_control":178,"n_intervention":177,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.31,"mean_intervention":0.33,"cohens_d":0.0429,"variance_d":0.0113,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":77,"study_id":132,"es_id":175,"reference":"Hallsworth et al. (2015)","title":"Stating appointment costs in SMS reminders reduces missed hospital appointments: Findings from two randomised controlled trials","year":2015,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":5125,"n_comparison":5125,"n_control":2566,"n_intervention":2559,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.111,"mean_intervention":0.098,"cohens_d":0.0425,"variance_d":0.0008,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":211,"study_id":330,"es_id":444,"reference":"Zikmund-Fisher et al. (2011)","title":"\"I'll do what they did\": Social norm information and cancer treatment decisions.","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1119,"n_comparison":746,"n_control":373,"n_intervention":373,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.269,"mean_intervention":0.251,"cohens_d":0.041,"variance_d":0.0054,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":100,"study_id":169,"es_id":222,"reference":"Korn et al. (2018)","title":"Social nudging: The effect of social feedback interventions on vaccine uptake","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":288,"n_comparison":288,"n_control":96,"n_intervention":192,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.5,"mean_intervention":0.52,"cohens_d":0.04,"variance_d":0.0156,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":151,"study_id":238,"es_id":324,"reference":"Rodriguez & Saavedra (2019)","title":"The persistent effects of youth savings reminders: Experimental evidence from text-message campaigns in Colombia","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":7795,"n_comparison":5200,"n_control":2780,"n_intervention":2420,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":11.9196,"sd_control":519.8261,"mean_intervention":32.89505,"sd_intervention":553.0142,"cohens_d":0.0392,"variance_d":0.0008,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":22,"study_id":35,"es_id":49,"reference":"BIT (2013)","title":"Applying behavioural insights to organ donation: Preliminary results from a randomised controlled trial","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":813990,"n_comparison":271330,"n_control":135665,"n_intervention":135665,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.02308,"mean_intervention":0.02907,"cohens_d":0.0376,"variance_d":0.00001,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":22,"study_id":35,"es_id":50,"reference":"BIT (2013)","title":"Applying behavioural insights to organ donation: Preliminary results from a randomised controlled trial","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":813990,"n_comparison":271330,"n_control":135665,"n_intervention":135665,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.02308,"mean_intervention":0.02907,"cohens_d":0.0376,"variance_d":0.00001,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":22,"study_id":35,"es_id":51,"reference":"BIT (2013)","title":"Applying behavioural insights to organ donation: Preliminary results from a randomised controlled trial","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":813990,"n_comparison":271330,"n_control":135665,"n_intervention":135665,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.02308,"mean_intervention":0.02907,"cohens_d":0.0376,"variance_d":0.00001,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":33,"study_id":55,"es_id":76,"reference":"Camilleri & Larrick (2014)","title":"Metric and scale design as choice architecture tools","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":484,"n_comparison":323,"n_control":162,"n_intervention":161,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.552,"mean_intervention":0.57,"cohens_d":0.0363,"variance_d":0.0124,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":211,"study_id":333,"es_id":445,"reference":"Zikmund-Fisher et al. (2011)","title":"\"I'll do what they did\": Social norm information and cancer treatment decisions.","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1077,"n_comparison":718,"n_control":359,"n_intervention":359,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.461,"mean_intervention":0.444,"cohens_d":0.0342,"variance_d":0.0056,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":20,"study_id":33,"es_id":40,"reference":"BETA (2019)","title":"Credit when it's due","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":16030,"n_comparison":8005,"n_control":4001,"n_intervention":4004,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":551,"sd_control":1759,"mean_intervention":616,"sd_intervention":2135,"cohens_d":0.0332,"variance_d":0.0005,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":75,"study_id":129,"es_id":169,"reference":"Grepin et al. (2019)","title":"Cash on delivery: Results of a randomized experiment to promote maternal health care in Kenya","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":1514,"n_comparison":908,"n_control":574,"n_intervention":334,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.3885,"mean_intervention":0.40419,"cohens_d":0.0321,"variance_d":0.0047,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":21,"study_id":34,"es_id":46,"reference":"Bhanot (2017)","title":"Cheap promises: Evidence from loan repayment pledges in an online experiment","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":2787,"n_comparison":1424,"n_control":729,"n_intervention":695,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.78,"mean_intervention":0.792,"cohens_d":0.0293,"variance_d":0.0028,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":21,"study_id":34,"es_id":47,"reference":"Bhanot (2017)","title":"Cheap promises: Evidence from loan repayment pledges in an online experiment","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":2787,"n_comparison":1404,"n_control":729,"n_intervention":675,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.56,"mean_intervention":0.574,"cohens_d":0.0283,"variance_d":0.0029,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":20,"study_id":33,"es_id":41,"reference":"BETA (2019)","title":"Credit when it's due","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":16030,"n_comparison":8018,"n_control":4001,"n_intervention":4017,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":551,"sd_control":1759,"mean_intervention":605,"sd_intervention":2162,"cohens_d":0.0274,"variance_d":0.0005,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":48,"study_id":79,"es_id":103,"reference":"Damgaard & Gravert (2017)","title":"The hidden costs of nudging: Experimental evidence from reminders in fundraising","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":17391,"n_comparison":17391,"n_control":8692,"n_intervention":8699,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.0035,"mean_intervention":0.0053,"cohens_d":0.0273,"variance_d":0.0002,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":87,"study_id":142,"es_id":186,"reference":"Impact and Innovation Unit (2018)","title":"Using behavioural insights to encourage charitable donations","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"consequence","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":196876,"n_comparison":56251,"n_control":28124,"n_intervention":28127,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.00021,"mean_intervention":0.00075,"cohens_d":0.0254,"variance_d":0.0001,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":19,"study_id":32,"es_id":37,"reference":"BETA (2018)","title":"Energy labels that make cents","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":39614,"n_comparison":26341,"n_control":13142,"n_intervention":13199,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.0101,"mean_intervention":0.0123,"cohens_d":0.0209,"variance_d":0.0002,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":19,"study_id":32,"es_id":38,"reference":"BETA (2018)","title":"Energy labels that make cents","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":39614,"n_comparison":26415,"n_control":13142,"n_intervention":13273,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.0101,"mean_intervention":0.0118,"cohens_d":0.0163,"variance_d":0.0002,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":39,"study_id":69,"es_id":90,"reference":"Castleman & Page (2016)","title":"Freshman year financial aid nudges: An experiment to increase FAFSA renewal and college persistence","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":808,"n_comparison":808,"n_control":395,"n_intervention":413,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.806,"mean_intervention":0.812,"cohens_d":0.0153,"variance_d":0.005,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":87,"study_id":142,"es_id":187,"reference":"Impact and Innovation Unit (2018)","title":"Using behavioural insights to encourage charitable donations","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":196876,"n_comparison":56250,"n_control":28124,"n_intervention":28126,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.00021,"mean_intervention":0.00043,"cohens_d":0.0121,"variance_d":0.0001,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":128,"study_id":206,"es_id":279,"reference":"Miller et al. (2016)","title":"The effects of pre-ordering and behavioral nudges on national school lunch program participants' food item selection","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":133,"n_comparison":133,"n_control":98,"n_intervention":35,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.762,"mean_intervention":0.764,"cohens_d":0.0047,"variance_d":0.0388,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":87,"study_id":142,"es_id":188,"reference":"Impact and Innovation Unit (2018)","title":"Using behavioural insights to encourage charitable donations","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":196876,"n_comparison":56249,"n_control":28124,"n_intervention":28125,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.00021,"mean_intervention":0.00025,"cohens_d":0.0023,"variance_d":0.0001,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":113,"study_id":187,"es_id":242,"reference":"Loibl et al. (2018)","title":"Testing strategies to increase saving in individual development account programs","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":105,"n_comparison":105,"n_control":54,"n_intervention":51,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.6319,"mean_intervention":0.632,"cohens_d":0.0002,"variance_d":0.0381,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":211,"study_id":332,"es_id":446,"reference":"Zikmund-Fisher et al. (2011)","title":"\"I'll do what they did\": Social norm information and cancer treatment decisions.","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1077,"n_comparison":718,"n_control":359,"n_intervention":359,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.266,"mean_intervention":0.266,"cohens_d":0,"variance_d":0.0056,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":73,"study_id":125,"es_id":167,"reference":"Goswami & Urminsky (2016)","title":"When should the ask be a nudge? The effect of default amounts on charitable donations","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":76,"n_comparison":38,"n_control":24,"n_intervention":14,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.21,"mean_intervention":0.21,"cohens_d":0,"variance_d":0.1131,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":87,"study_id":142,"es_id":189,"reference":"Impact and Innovation Unit (2018)","title":"Using behavioural insights to encourage charitable donations","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":196876,"n_comparison":56249,"n_control":28124,"n_intervention":28125,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.00021,"mean_intervention":0.00018,"cohens_d":-0.0025,"variance_d":0.0001,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":76,"study_id":130,"es_id":171,"reference":"Hainmueller et al. (2018)","title":"A randomized controlled design reveals barriers to citizenship for low-income immigrants","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":1190,"n_comparison":579,"n_control":425,"n_intervention":154,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.44,"mean_intervention":0.438,"cohens_d":-0.004,"variance_d":0.0088,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":87,"study_id":142,"es_id":190,"reference":"Impact and Innovation Unit (2018)","title":"Using behavioural insights to encourage charitable donations","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":196876,"n_comparison":56248,"n_control":28124,"n_intervention":28124,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.00021,"mean_intervention":0.00014,"cohens_d":-0.0054,"variance_d":0.0001,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":22,"study_id":35,"es_id":52,"reference":"BIT (2013)","title":"Applying behavioural insights to organ donation: Preliminary results from a randomised controlled trial","year":2013,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":813990,"n_comparison":271330,"n_control":135665,"n_intervention":135665,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.02308,"mean_intervention":0.0221,"cohens_d":-0.0066,"variance_d":0.00001,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":69,"study_id":121,"es_id":155,"reference":"Goldzahl et al. (2018)","title":"Increasing breast-cancer screening uptake: A randomized controlled experiment","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":15880,"n_comparison":10584,"n_control":5277,"n_intervention":5307,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.4758,"mean_intervention":0.4709,"cohens_d":-0.0098,"variance_d":0.0004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":203,"study_id":318,"es_id":425,"reference":"Wansink et al. (2017)","title":"Larger partitions lead to larger sales: Divided grocery carts alter purchase norms and increase sales","year":2017,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":83,"n_comparison":52,"n_control":26,"n_intervention":26,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":8.77,"sd_control":4.39,"mean_intervention":8.72,"sd_intervention":4.42,"cohens_d":-0.0114,"variance_d":0.0769,"approximation":false,"wansink":true} {"publication_id":87,"study_id":142,"es_id":191,"reference":"Impact and Innovation Unit (2018)","title":"Using behavioural insights to encourage charitable donations","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":196876,"n_comparison":56249,"n_control":28124,"n_intervention":28125,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.00021,"mean_intervention":0.00007,"cohens_d":-0.0123,"variance_d":0.0001,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":37,"study_id":65,"es_id":86,"reference":"Carter & Gonzalez-Vallejo (2018)","title":"Nutrient-specific system versus full fact panel: Testing the benefits of nutrient-specific front-of-package labels in a student sample","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":174,"n_comparison":174,"n_control":83,"n_intervention":91,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":29.71,"sd_control":16.92,"mean_intervention":29.51,"sd_intervention":15.51,"cohens_d":-0.0123,"variance_d":0.023,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":27,"study_id":47,"es_id":64,"reference":"Bronchetti et al. (2011)","title":"When a nudge isn't enough: Defaults and saving among low-income tax filers","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":259,"n_comparison":259,"n_control":130,"n_intervention":129,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.09555,"mean_intervention":0.09045,"cohens_d":-0.0176,"variance_d":0.0154,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":74,"study_id":128,"es_id":168,"reference":"Grant et al. (2018)","title":"A comparison of the efficacy of three intervention trail types: Postal, group, and one-to-one facilitation, prior management and the impact of message framing and repeat meassages on the flock prevalence of lameness in sheep","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":660,"n_comparison":660,"n_control":329,"n_intervention":331,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.435,"mean_intervention":0.426,"cohens_d":-0.0182,"variance_d":0.0061,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":129,"study_id":207,"es_id":280,"reference":"Missbach & Konig (2016)","title":"Middle choice preference and snack choice: The role of self-regulatory resources to nudge healthier food choice","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"effort","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":182,"n_comparison":182,"n_control":89,"n_intervention":93,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.35,"mean_intervention":0.34,"cohens_d":-0.021,"variance_d":0.022,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":69,"study_id":121,"es_id":156,"reference":"Goldzahl et al. (2018)","title":"Increasing breast-cancer screening uptake: A randomized controlled experiment","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":15880,"n_comparison":10573,"n_control":5277,"n_intervention":5296,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.4758,"mean_intervention":0.4651,"cohens_d":-0.0214,"variance_d":0.0004,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":89,"study_id":149,"es_id":198,"reference":"Jin (2011)","title":"Improving response rates in web surveys with default setting: The effects of default on web survey participation and permission","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":124,"n_comparison":124,"n_control":62,"n_intervention":62,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.625,"mean_intervention":0.613,"cohens_d":-0.0247,"variance_d":0.0323,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":139,"study_id":221,"es_id":300,"reference":"Park et al. (2010)","title":"A randomized evaluation of loss and gain frames in an invitation to screening for type 2 diabetes","year":2010,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":116,"n_comparison":116,"n_control":57,"n_intervention":59,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.82,"mean_intervention":0.81,"cohens_d":-0.0258,"variance_d":0.0345,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":134,"study_id":215,"es_id":293,"reference":"Niven et al. (2019)","title":"Effects of health star labelling on the healthiness of adults' fast food meal selections: An experimental study","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1007,"n_comparison":505,"n_control":252,"n_intervention":253,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":3.68,"sd_control":3.88764,"mean_intervention":3.78,"sd_intervention":3.81419,"cohens_d":-0.026,"variance_d":0.0079,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":45,"study_id":76,"es_id":97,"reference":"Courtright et al. (2016)","title":"A randomized trial of expanding choice sets to motivate advance directive completion","year":2016,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":316,"n_comparison":316,"n_control":160,"n_intervention":156,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.131,"mean_intervention":0.122,"cohens_d":-0.0271,"variance_d":0.0127,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":38,"study_id":68,"es_id":89,"reference":"Castleman & Page (2015)","title":"Summer nudging: Can personalized text messages and peer mentor outreach increase college going among low-income high school graduates?","year":2015,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":1393,"n_comparison":1393,"n_control":696,"n_intervention":697,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.701,"mean_intervention":0.685,"cohens_d":-0.0347,"variance_d":0.0029,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":75,"study_id":129,"es_id":170,"reference":"Grepin et al. (2019)","title":"Cash on delivery: Results of a randomized experiment to promote maternal health care in Kenya","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":1514,"n_comparison":1180,"n_control":574,"n_intervention":606,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.3885,"mean_intervention":0.37129,"cohens_d":-0.0355,"variance_d":0.0034,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":173,"study_id":269,"es_id":363,"reference":"Soon et al. (2018)","title":"Effect of two behavioural 'nudging' interventions on management decisions for low back pain: A randomised vignette-based study in general practitioners","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":120,"n_comparison":61,"n_control":30,"n_intervention":31,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.425,"mean_intervention":0.4509,"cohens_d":-0.0522,"variance_d":0.0656,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":58,"study_id":100,"es_id":132,"reference":"Evans et al. (2011)","title":"Trust and self-control: The moderating role of the default","year":2011,"location":"inside US","domain":"finance","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":62,"n_comparison":62,"n_control":36,"n_intervention":26,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":9.61,"sd_control":6.85,"mean_intervention":9.15,"sd_intervention":6.09,"cohens_d":-0.0703,"variance_d":0.0663,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":76,"study_id":130,"es_id":172,"reference":"Hainmueller et al. (2018)","title":"A randomized controlled design reveals barriers to citizenship for low-income immigrants","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":1190,"n_comparison":739,"n_control":425,"n_intervention":314,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.44,"mean_intervention":0.404,"cohens_d":-0.0729,"variance_d":0.0055,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":72,"study_id":124,"es_id":160,"reference":"Gopalan et al. (2014)","title":"Translating the hemoglobin A1C with more easily understood feedback: A randomized controlled trial","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":177,"n_comparison":116,"n_control":58,"n_intervention":58,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":-0.74,"sd_control":2.81784,"mean_intervention":-0.55,"sd_intervention":2.28473,"cohens_d":-0.0741,"variance_d":0.0345,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":24,"study_id":39,"es_id":56,"reference":"Bohm et al. (2016)","title":"Outcome valence and externality valence framing in public good dilemmas","year":2016,"location":"outside US","domain":"pro-social","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":160,"n_comparison":160,"n_control":80,"n_intervention":80,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":6.15,"sd_control":3.83,"mean_intervention":5.92,"sd_intervention":2.05,"cohens_d":-0.0749,"variance_d":0.025,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":134,"study_id":215,"es_id":294,"reference":"Niven et al. (2019)","title":"Effects of health star labelling on the healthiness of adults' fast food meal selections: An experimental study","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":1007,"n_comparison":503,"n_control":252,"n_intervention":251,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":5452,"sd_control":2790.188,"mean_intervention":5686,"sd_intervention":3067.556,"cohens_d":-0.0798,"variance_d":0.008,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":35,"study_id":62,"es_id":83,"reference":"Carrera et al. (2018)","title":"The limits of simple implementation intentions: Evidence from a field experiment on making plans to exercise","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":877,"n_comparison":877,"n_control":439,"n_intervention":438,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2.63,"sd_control":3.15,"mean_intervention":2.33,"sd_intervention":2.92,"cohens_d":-0.0988,"variance_d":0.0046,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":210,"study_id":329,"es_id":437,"reference":"Zeinstra et al. (2010)","title":"Offering choice and its effect on Dutch children's liking and consumption of vegetables: A randomized controlled trial","year":2010,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":303,"n_comparison":206,"n_control":96,"n_intervention":110,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":55.8,"sd_control":45,"mean_intervention":51.3,"sd_intervention":46,"cohens_d":-0.0988,"variance_d":0.0195,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":147,"study_id":233,"es_id":315,"reference":"Raynor & Wing (2007)","title":"Package unit size and amount of food: Do both influence intake?","year":2007,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":28,"n_comparison":28,"n_control":14,"n_intervention":14,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":4026.8,"sd_control":2313.1,"mean_intervention":3783.4,"sd_intervention":2335.1,"cohens_d":-0.1047,"variance_d":0.1431,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":80,"study_id":135,"es_id":178,"reference":"Harnack et al. (2008)","title":"Effects of calorie labeling and value size pricing on fast food meal choices: Results from an experimental trial","year":2008,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":301,"n_comparison":301,"n_control":150,"n_intervention":151,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":827.5,"sd_control":400.6,"mean_intervention":873.6,"sd_intervention":439.1,"cohens_d":-0.1097,"variance_d":0.0133,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":186,"study_id":293,"es_id":392,"reference":"Thunstrom et al. (2018)","title":"Nudges that hurt those already hurting: Distributional and unintended effects of salience nudges","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":410,"n_comparison":234,"n_control":165,"n_intervention":69,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2.189,"sd_control":2.195,"mean_intervention":1.919,"sd_intervention":2.271,"cohens_d":-0.1218,"variance_d":0.0206,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":76,"study_id":130,"es_id":173,"reference":"Hainmueller et al. (2018)","title":"A randomized controlled design reveals barriers to citizenship for low-income immigrants","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"other","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"natural_field","population":"adults","n_study":1190,"n_comparison":722,"n_control":425,"n_intervention":297,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.44,"mean_intervention":0.38,"cohens_d":-0.1221,"variance_d":0.0057,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":55,"study_id":93,"es_id":124,"reference":"Dos Santos et al. (2019)","title":"Impact of nudging intervention and factors associated with vegetable dish choice among European adolescents","year":2019,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"reminder","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":84,"n_comparison":84,"n_control":47,"n_intervention":37,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.1489,"mean_intervention":0.1081,"cohens_d":-0.1223,"variance_d":0.0484,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":32,"study_id":53,"es_id":72,"reference":"Byrd et al. (2018)","title":"Adding sodium information to casual dining restaurant menus: Beneficial or detrimental for consumers?","year":2018,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"visibility","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":104,"n_comparison":104,"n_control":57,"n_intervention":47,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":3041,"sd_control":1317.144,"mean_intervention":3220,"sd_intervention":1316.56,"cohens_d":-0.1359,"variance_d":0.0389,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":121,"study_id":198,"es_id":264,"reference":"McCaul & Kopp (1982)","title":"Effects of goal setting and commitment on increasing metal recycling","year":1982,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"assistance","intervention_technique":"commitment","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":55,"n_comparison":55,"n_control":28,"n_intervention":27,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":45.28,"sd_control":36.05017,"mean_intervention":40.87,"sd_intervention":24.20288,"cohens_d":-0.1431,"variance_d":0.0729,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":148,"study_id":234,"es_id":316,"reference":"Reiter et al. (2012)","title":"Default policies and parents' consent for school-located HPV vaccination","year":2012,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":188,"n_comparison":188,"n_control":94,"n_intervention":94,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.41,"mean_intervention":0.34,"cohens_d":-0.1447,"variance_d":0.0213,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":210,"study_id":329,"es_id":438,"reference":"Zeinstra et al. (2010)","title":"Offering choice and its effect on Dutch children's liking and consumption of vegetables: A randomized controlled trial","year":2010,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"children and/or adolescents","n_study":303,"n_comparison":193,"n_control":96,"n_intervention":97,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":55.8,"sd_control":45,"mean_intervention":48.5,"sd_intervention":47,"cohens_d":-0.1586,"variance_d":0.0208,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":43,"study_id":74,"es_id":95,"reference":"Cheung et al. (2017)","title":"The Hunger Games: Using hunger to promote healthy choices in self-control conflicts","year":2017,"location":"inside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":201,"n_comparison":201,"n_control":101,"n_intervention":100,"binary_outcome":false,"cohens_d":-0.1636,"variance_d":0.02,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":9,"study_id":10,"es_id":14,"reference":"Bacon & Krpan (2018)","title":"(Not) eating for the environment: The impact of restaurant menu design on vegetarian food choice","year":2018,"location":"outside US","domain":"food","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"composition","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":565,"n_comparison":380,"n_control":194,"n_intervention":186,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.13,"mean_intervention":0.06,"cohens_d":-0.2428,"variance_d":0.0106,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":148,"study_id":235,"es_id":317,"reference":"Reiter et al. (2012)","title":"Default policies and parents' consent for school-located HPV vaccination","year":2012,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"structure","intervention_technique":"default","type_experiment":"artefactual_field","population":"adults","n_study":80,"n_comparison":80,"n_control":40,"n_intervention":40,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.75,"mean_intervention":0.52,"cohens_d":-0.4836,"variance_d":0.0515,"approximation":true,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":207,"study_id":324,"es_id":432,"reference":"Yeomans & Herberich (2014)","title":"An experimental test of the effect of negative social norms on energy-efficient investments","year":2014,"location":"inside US","domain":"environment","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"social_reference","type_experiment":"framed_field","population":"adults","n_study":67,"n_comparison":67,"n_control":31,"n_intervention":36,"binary_outcome":true,"mean_control":0.581,"mean_intervention":0.278,"cohens_d":-0.6228,"variance_d":0.0629,"approximation":false,"wansink":false} {"publication_id":105,"study_id":177,"es_id":230,"reference":"Lalor & Hailey (1989)","title":"The effects of message framing and feelings of susceptibility to breast cancer on reported frequency of breast self-examination","year":1989,"location":"inside US","domain":"health","intervention_category":"information","intervention_technique":"translation","type_experiment":"conventional_lab","population":"adults","n_study":23,"n_comparison":23,"n_control":12,"n_intervention":11,"binary_outcome":false,"mean_control":2.46,"sd_control":1.2,"mean_intervention":3.33,"sd_intervention":1.32,"cohens_d":-0.6913,"variance_d":0.1846,"approximation":true,"wansink":false}